Analysis | First level contrast | Second level contrast | Matched for | Stats | Notes | Findings | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Acc | RT | ||||||
Weiller et al. (1995): Vox 1 |
Verb generation vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox NDC |
Behavioral data notes: in practice trials, patients produced 1.5 words on average per prompt, not all of which were verbs, while controls 2.3 words on average per prompt, almost all of which were verbs; search volume: perisylvian; software: SPM; qualitative comparison on p. 729 (the word "significant" is used) | ↑ R IFG ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG notes: based more on Figure 2 than the text |
Weiller et al. (1995): Vox 2 |
Pseudoword repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
AS | UNR | Vox NDC |
Behavioral data notes: all participants are reported to have had no difficulties in performing the repetition task; search volume: perisylvian; software: SPM; qualitative comparison on p. 729 (the word "significant" is used) | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R IFG ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG notes: based more on Figure 2 than the text |
Belin et al. (1996): ROI 1 |
Word repetition with MIT-like intonation vs word repetition | CB Aphasia |
NBD | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Behavioral data notes: more words were correctly repeated with MIT (16.3 ± 8) than without (12.4 ± 8; p < 0.03); number of ROIs: 18; ROIs: (1) L Broca's area; (2) L prefrontal; (3) L sensorimotor mouth; (4) L parietal; (5) L Wernicke's area; (6) L Heschl's gyrus; (7) L anterior STG; (8) L MTG; (9) L temporal pole; (10-18) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; activation quantified as mean rCBF, not including any intersection of the infarct with the ROI; three left hemisphere ROIs were excluded (3, 6, 9) because they were completely infarcted in 4 or more patients | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R posterior STG |
Ohyama et al. (1996): ROI 1 |
Word repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: some of the patients made a few errors, so as a group they may have been less accurate than controls; number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA; how ROIs defined: spheres around control peaks; the rCBF increase in R PIF was also significant at p < 0.005 for nonfluent patients with Fisher's protected least-significant difference | ↑ R IFG ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Ohyama et al. (1996): ROI 2 |
Word repetition vs rest | CAA Aphasia fluent (n = 10) vs non-fluent (n = 6) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA; how ROIs defined: spheres around control peaks | ↓ R IFG |
Ohyama et al. (1996): ROI 3 |
Word repetition vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: spontaneous speech (WAB) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA; how ROIs defined: spheres around control peaks; no correction for multiple comparisons across WAB subscores | ↑ L IFG |
Ohyama et al. (1996): ROI 4 |
Word repetition vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: comprehension (WAB) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA; how ROIs defined: spheres around control peaks; this non-significant finding is implied but not stated explicitly | None |
Ohyama et al. (1996): ROI 5 |
Word repetition vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: repetition (WAB) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA; how ROIs defined: spheres around control peaks; this non-significant finding is implied but not stated explicitly | None |
Ohyama et al. (1996): ROI 6 |
Word repetition vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: naming (WAB) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA; how ROIs defined: spheres around control peaks; this non-significant finding is implied but not stated explicitly | None |
Heiss et al. (1997): Vox 1 |
Word repetition vs rest | LAA (Aphasia with good recovery (n = 3) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with poor recovery (n = 3) T2 vs T1) Somewhat valid (TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
UNR | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: not stated; qualitative generalization across individuals on pp. 214-6 | ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ R posterior STG/STS/MTG notes: the consistent aspects of the findings were that there was an emergence of L posterior temporal activation in patients with better recovery, and R posterior temporal activation in patients with worse recovery |
Heiss et al. (1997): ROI 1 |
Word repetition vs rest | LAA (Aphasia with good recovery (n = 3) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with poor recovery (n = 3) T2 vs T1) Somewhat valid (TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) L superior temporal cortex; (2) R superior temporal cortex; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; activation quantified in terms of extent exceeding 10% signal change, and mean % increase over the activation; qualitative generalization across individuals on pp. 214, 216 | ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus |
Karbe et al. (1998): ROI 1 |
Word repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 8; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) L STG/HG; (3) L SMA; (4) L ventral precentral; (5-8) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 219, but only the L SMA comparison is explicitly quantified | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus |
Karbe et al. (1998): ROI 2 |
Word repetition vs rest | CC Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T1 (n = 7) Covariate: TT T1 Somewhat valid (TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 8; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) L STG/HG; (3) L SMA; (4) L ventral precentral; (5-8) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images | None |
Karbe et al. (1998): ROI 3 |
Word repetition vs rest | CC Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T2 (n = 7) Covariate: TT T2 Somewhat valid (TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 8; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) L STG/HG; (3) L SMA; (4) L ventral precentral; (5-8) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images | ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus notes: more activation in patients with more severe aphasia per TT |
Karbe et al. (1998): ROI 4 |
Word repetition vs rest | LC Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) (n = 7) T2 vs T1 Covariate: subsequent outcome (T2) TT Not valid (the logic behind correlating activation changes and language outcome is unclear; TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L STG/HG; how ROI defined: individual anatomical images | ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus notes: increase in activation for repetition was correlated with better aphasia outcome per TT |
Karbe et al. (1998): ROI 5 |
Word repetition vs rest | CC Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T2 (n = 7) Covariate: previous Δ (T2 vs T1) activation in L STG/HG Not valid (logically problematic because patients with less severe initial aphasia would also be expected to show little L temporal increase, but would not be expected to show R temporal recruitment) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) R IFG; (2) R STG/HG; (3) R SMA; (4) R ventral precentral; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images | ↓ R IFG ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus notes: patients with more increase in L STG/HG activation showed less activation of R hemisphere regions at T2 |
Cao et al. (1999): ROI 1 |
Picture naming vs viewing nonsense drawings | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L IFG and MFG; (2) L pSTG, AG and SMG; (3) R IFG and MFG; (4) R pSTG, AG and SMG; (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; how ROIs defined: (1-4) individual anatomical images; activation quantified in terms of extent | ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG ↓ LI (frontal) ↓ LI (temporal) |
Cao et al. (1999): ROI 2 |
Picture naming vs viewing nonsense drawings | CC Aphasia Covariate: picture naming (outside scanner) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L IFG and MFG; (2) L pSTG, AG and SMG; (3) R IFG and MFG; (4) R pSTG, AG and SMG; (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; how ROIs defined: (1-4) individual anatomical images; activation quantified in terms of extent | ↑ LI (frontal) |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 1 |
Noun repetition vs rest | LA Aphasia with subcortical damage (n = 9) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R IFG pars opercularis |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 2 |
Noun repetition vs rest | LA Aphasia with frontal damage (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R IFG pars opercularis |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 3 |
Noun repetition vs rest | LA Aphasia with temporal damage (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R mid temporal ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 4 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAA Aphasia with temporal damage T1 (n = 7) vs with subcortical damage T1 (n = 9) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L posterior STG ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 5 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAA Aphasia with temporal damage T1 (n = 7) vs with frontal damage T1 (n = 7) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 6 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAA Aphasia with temporal damage T2 (n = 7) vs with subcortical damage T2 (n = 9) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 7 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAA Aphasia with temporal damage T2 (n = 7) vs with frontal damage T2 (n = 7) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 8 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia with subcortical damage T1 (n = 9) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 9 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia with frontal damage T1 (n = 7) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 10 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia with temporal damage T1 (n = 7) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434; L IFG pars opercularis noted as different in text despite being significant in both groups | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R mid temporal |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 11 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia with subcortical damage T2 (n = 9) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L Heschl's gyrus |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 12 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia with frontal damage T2 (n = 7) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L Heschl's gyrus |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 13 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAC Aphasia with temporal damage T2 (n = 7) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 434 | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 14 |
Noun repetition vs rest | LA Aphasia with subcortical or frontal damage and good recovery (n = 11) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on pp. 434-5 | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R IFG pars opercularis |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 15 |
Noun repetition vs rest | LA Aphasia with subcortical or frontal damage and poor recovery (n = 5) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on pp. 434-5 | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R IFG pars opercularis |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 16 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAA Aphasia with subcortical and frontal damage and good recovery T1 (n = 11) vs with subcortical and frontal damage and poor recovery T1 (n = 5) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 435 | ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L mid temporal |
Heiss et al. (1999): ROI 17 |
Noun repetition vs rest | CAA Aphasia with subcortical and frontal damage and good recovery T2 (n = 11) vs with subcortical and frontal damage and poor recovery T2 (n = 5) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NDC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images; qualitative comparison on p. 435 | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction |
Kessler et al. (2000): ROI 1 |
Word repetition vs rest | LA Aphasia treated with pirecetam (n = 12) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L BA 44; (2) L BA 45; (3) L ventral PrCG; (4) L HG; (5) L BA 41 and 42; (6) L BA 22; (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus |
Kessler et al. (2000): ROI 2 |
Word repetition vs rest | LA Aphasia treated with placebo (n = 12) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L BA 44; (2) L BA 45; (3) L ventral PrCG; (4) L HG; (5) L BA 41 and 42; (6) L BA 22; (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual anatomical images | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction |
Rosen et al. (2000): Vox 1 |
Word stem completion (PET) vs rest (PET) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | Y | Vox U |
Search volume: whole brain; software: not stated; correction for multiple comparisons unclear; there may be circularity in only correcting for the number of regions that seemed to show differences | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R IFG ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ L IFG |
Rosen et al. (2000): Vox 2 |
Word stem completion (fMRI) vs rest (fMRI) | CAC Aphasia (n = 5) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: not stated; qualitative comparison on p. 1888 | ↑ R IFG ↓ L IFG |
Rosen et al. (2000): ROI 1 |
Word stem completion (fMRI) vs rest (fMRI) | CAC Aphasia (n = 5) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) R IFG; (2) SMA; how ROIs defined: not stated but seem to be functional; possibly circular because not clear how ROIs defined | ↑ R IFG |
Blasi et al. (2002): Vox 1 |
Word stem completion (novel items) vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | N | Vox U |
Behavioral data notes: covert task but overt data acquired separately; patients less accurate and slower than controls; search volume: whole brain; software: not stated; voxelwise p: ~.001 (z > 3); cluster extent cutoff: 45 voxels (size not stated); Monte Carlo analysis not described in detail; rather than fitting a HRF, the authors looked at the shape of the signal in the 8 volumes following each stimulus | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R dorsal precentral ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction notes: labels based on coordinates reported |
Blasi et al. (2002): ROI 1 |
Word stem completion (novel items) vs word stem completion (repeated items) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
Y | Y | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: covert task but overt data acquired separately; no interaction of group by practice for accuracy or RT; number of ROIs: 14; ROIs: (1) L dorsal IFG; (2) L ventral IFG; (3) R MFG; (4) L anterior fusiform; (5) R anterior fusiform; (6) R posterior fusiform; (7) R lateral occipital; (8) R lateral cerebellum; (9) L SMA; (10) R dorsal IFG; (11) R posterior fusiform; (12) R lateral occipital; (13) R lingual; (14) L MTG; how ROIs defined: regions that were active for the main effect of word stem completion (irrespective of practice) in either group and modulated by practice in that group; circular because ROIs defined in one group or the other; the L ROIs showed repetition suppression in controls but not in patients, and this difference is interpreted by the authors, but not supported statistically | ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ L IFG ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus notes: labels based on coordinates reported |
Leff et al. (2002): Vox 1 |
Higher word rates vs lower word rates | CAC Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM99; qualitative comparison on p. 555; a FWE-corrected SPM is reported of the relationship in the 6 patients with L pSTS damage (Fig. 2), however it is masked in a way that is not explained (see figure caption), and there is no direct comparison between patients with L pSTS damage and controls | ↑ R posterior STS |
Leff et al. (2002): Vox 2 |
Higher word rates vs lower word rates | CAA Aphasia with pSTS (n = 6) damage vs without pSTS damage (n = 9) |
NANB | NANT | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM99; qualitative comparison on p. 555; a FWE-corrected SPM is reported of the relationship in the 6 patients with L pSTS damage (Fig. 2), however it is masked in a way that is not explained (see figure caption), and there is no direct comparison between patients with L pSTS damage and patients with R pSTS damage | ↑ R posterior STS |
Leff et al. (2002): ROI 1 |
Higher word rates vs lower word rates | CAC Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs control (n = 8) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R pSTS; how ROI defined: the peak voxel for the contrast in the R pSTS from each subject's individual analysis, but the search region is not stated; the controls and patients without pSTS damage were combined, however it is stated in the caption to Figure 2 that the patients with pSTS damage were significantly different to both | ↑ R posterior STS |
Leff et al. (2002): ROI 2 |
Higher word rates vs lower word rates | CAA Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs aphasia without pSTS damage (n = 9) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R pSTS; how ROI defined: the peak voxel for the contrast in the R pSTS from each subject's individual analysis, but the search region is not stated; the controls and patients without pSTS damage were combined, however it is stated in the caption to Figure 2 that the patients with pSTS damage were significantly different to both | ↑ R posterior STS |
Blank et al. (2003): Vox 1 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CAC Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs control |
N | NANT | Vox SVC |
Behavioral data notes: word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis notes: no voxels survived FWE correction without SVC |
Blank et al. (2003): Vox 2 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CAC Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs control |
N | NANT | Vox SVC |
Behavioral data notes: word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis |
Blank et al. (2003): Vox 3 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CAA Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs without IFG POp damage (n = 7) |
N | NANT | Vox SVC |
Behavioral data notes: word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis | None notes: patients with L IFG POp damage showed numerically more signal in the R IFG POp |
Blank et al. (2003): Vox 4 |
Propositional speech production vs counting | CAC Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs control |
N | NANT | Vox SVC |
Behavioral data notes: word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis | None |
Blank et al. (2003): Vox 5 |
Propositional speech production vs counting | CAC Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs control |
N | NANT | Vox SVC |
Behavioral data notes: word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis | None |
Blank et al. (2003): Vox 6 |
Propositional speech production vs counting | CAA Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs without IFG POp damage (n = 7) |
N | NANT | Vox SVC |
Behavioral data notes: word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis | None |
Blank et al. (2003): ROI 1 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) Covariate: speech rate during scan |
UNR | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R IFG pars opercularis; how ROI defined: defined by flipping L IFG pars opercularis activation in controls | None |
Blank et al. (2003): ROI 2 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 7) Covariate: speech rate during scan |
UNR | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R IFG pars opercularis; how ROI defined: defined by flipping L IFG pars opercularis activation in controls | None |
Blank et al. (2003): ROI 3 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) Covariate: four different QPA measures |
UNR | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R IFG pars opercularis; how ROI defined: defined by flipping L IFG pars opercularis activation in controls | None |
Cardebat et al. (2003): Vox 1 |
Word generation vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
N | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated); nature of inclusive masks unclear | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R cerebellum notes: based on Figure 2 |
Cardebat et al. (2003): Vox 2 |
Word generation vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ word generation accuracy |
C | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 100 voxels (size not stated); nature of inclusive masks unclear | ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L occipital ↓ L hippocampus/MTL ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R occipital |
Sharp et al. (2004): Vox 1 |
Semantic decision vs syllable count decision | CAC Aphasia vs control (clear speech) |
AM | Y | Vox SVC |
Behavioral data notes: interaction of group by task not reported for accuracy; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 with SVC in fusiform gyri, temporal poles, L IFG, L orbitofrontal and L SFG | ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Sharp et al. (2004): Vox 2 |
Semantic decision vs syllable count decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: semantic decision accuracy |
C | UNR | Vox SVC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 with SVC in fusiform gyri, temporal poles, L IFG, L orbitofrontal and L SFG; fixed effects; this analysis is not clearly described | ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus notes: patients who were more accurate had more activity in R anterior fusiform gyrus |
Sharp et al. (2004): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision vs syllable count decision | CAC Aphasia vs control (clear speech) |
AM | Y | ROI Anat One |
Behavioral data notes: interaction of group by task not reported for accuracy; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L fusiform gyrus; how ROI defined: probabilistic brain atlas | ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Sharp et al. (2004): ROI 2 |
Semantic decision vs syllable count decision | CAC Aphasia vs control (noise vocoded) |
NAM | Y | ROI Anat One |
Behavioral data notes: patients were more accurate on semantic decisions than syllable decisions, whereas controls were less accurate on noise vocoded semantic decisions than clear syllable decisions (which were the baseline for this analysis); number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L fusiform gyrus; how ROI defined: probabilistic brain atlas | None notes: this analysis suggests that the difference between groups in the L fusiform gyrus disappears when the controls perform a semantic task that is similarly challenging |
Zahn et al. (2004): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision vs phonetic decision and lexical decision (conjunction) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNT | UNR | ROI LI One |
Behavioral data notes: relative performance on language and control tasks unclear; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: language network LI; conjunction analyses not clearly described; in two patients, a different conjunction was used (lexical decision vs phonetic decision & semantic decision vs phonetic decision) | None notes: LI > 0 in 12 out of 14 controls and 5 out of 7 patients; no significant difference |
Crinion & Price (2005): Vox 1 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia without temporal lobe damage (n = 9) vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox VFWC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05; cluster extent cutoff: 5 voxels (size not stated) | ↓ L dorsal precentral ↓ R somato-motor |
Crinion & Price (2005): Vox 2 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia with temporal lobe damage (n = 8) vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox VFWC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05; cluster extent cutoff: 5 voxels (size not stated) | ↓ L posterior STS ↓ L mid temporal |
Crinion & Price (2005): Vox 3 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia with temporal lobe damage (n = 8) vs without temporal lobe damage (n = 9) |
NANB | NANT | Vox VFWC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05; cluster extent cutoff: 5 voxels (size not stated) | ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L mid temporal |
Crinion & Price (2005): Vox 4 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia without temporal lobe damage (n = 9) Covariate: sentence comprehension (CAT) |
NANB | NANT | Vox VFWC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05; cluster extent cutoff: 5 voxels (size not stated); conjunction with main effect of story comprehension (details hard to follow); this was a multiple regression also involving patients with temporal lobe damage | ↑ L posterior STS ↑ R mid temporal notes: patients with better sentence comprehension had more activation in the L posterior STS and R mid STS |
Crinion & Price (2005): Vox 5 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia with temporal lobe damage (n = 8) Covariate: sentence comprehension (CAT) |
NANB | NANT | Vox VFWC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05; cluster extent cutoff: 5 voxels (size not stated); conjunction with main effect of story comprehension (details hard to follow); this was a multiple regression also involving patients without temporal lobe damage | ↑ R mid temporal notes: patients with better sentence comprehension had more activation in the R mid STS |
Crinion & Price (2005): Cplx 1 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia with temporal damage (n = 8) vs without temporal damage (n = 9) |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
Correlations were computed between activity in each voxel, and the sentence comprehension measure from the CAT, and were compared between the two aphasia groups, in regions with a main effect of story comprehension. The voxelwise threshold was p < .001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons. | Other: Activity in the L posterior STS was positively correlated with sentence comprehension in patients without temporal lobe damage, but not in patients with temporal lobe damage |
Crinion & Price (2005): Cplx 2 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia without temporal damage (n = 9) vs control |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
Correlations were computed between activity in each voxel, and post-scan story recall, and were compared between patients without temporal damage and controls, in regions with a main effect of story comprehension. The threshold was p < 0.05 corrected, plus a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels. | None |
Crinion & Price (2005): Cplx 3 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia with temporal damage (n = 8) vs control |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
Correlations were computed between activity in each voxel, and post-scan story recall, and were compared between patients with temporal damage and controls, in regions with a main effect of story comprehension. The threshold was p < 0.05 corrected, plus a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels. | None |
Crinion & Price (2005): Cplx 4 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia with temporal damage (n = 8) vs without temporal damage (n = 9) |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
Correlations were computed between activity in each voxel, and post-scan story recall, and were compared between the two aphasia groups, in regions with a main effect of story comprehension. The threshold was p < 0.05 corrected, plus a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels. | None |
de Boissezon et al. (2005): Vox 1 |
Word generation vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: time post onset |
Y | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: no significant correlation between time post onset and accuracy; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital notes: more activity with longer time post onset; based on coordinates in Table 3a |
de Boissezon et al. (2005): Vox 2 |
Word generation vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: word generation accuracy T1 |
C | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L precuneus ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ R insula ↑ R posterior STG notes: based on coordinates in Table 3b |
de Boissezon et al. (2005): Vox 3 |
Word generation vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
N | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 100 voxels (size not stated); description of masking unclear, but seems to be inclusively masked with T1, which seems inappropriate | ↑ L insula ↑ L posterior STG ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R cerebellum notes: based on coordinates in Table 2 |
de Boissezon et al. (2005): Vox 4 |
Word generation vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ word generation accuracy |
C | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 20 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R cerebellum notes: based on coordinates in Table 3c |
Connor et al. (2006): Vox 1 |
Word stem completion (novel items) vs word stem completion (repeated items) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
Y | Y | Vox NDC |
Behavioral data notes: covert task but overt data acquired separately; no interaction of group by practice for accuracy or RT; search volume: cerebellum; software: not stated; qualitative comparison on p. 174; Monte Carlo-based thresholding not described; rather than fitting a HRF, the authors looked at the shape of the signal in the 8 volumes following each stimulus | ↑ L cerebellum ↓ R cerebellum |
Connor et al. (2006): ROI 1 |
Word stem completion (novel items) vs word stem completion (repeated items) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
Y | Y | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: covert task but overt data acquired separately; no interaction of group by practice for accuracy or RT; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L cerebellum; how ROI defined: L cerebellar region with a learning effect in the patients; circular because ROIs defined in one group; rather than fitting a HRF, the authors looked at the shape of the signal in the 8 volumes following each stimulus | ↑ L cerebellum |
Crinion et al. (2006): Vox 1 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox VFWE |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Crinion et al. (2006): Vox 2 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia without temporal lobe damage (n = 6) vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox VFWE |
Search volume: voxels spared in all included patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Crinion et al. (2006): Vox 3 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia with temporal lobe damage (n = 18) vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox VFWE |
Search volume: voxels spared in all included patients; software: SPM99; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Crinion et al. (2006): ROI 1 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia with no temporal damage (excluding 1 with missing behavioral data and 1 outlier) or posterior temporal damage sparing anterior temporal cortex (n = 13) Covariate: auditory sentence comprehension (CAT) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ATL; how ROI defined: activation in the control group; same result obtained with or without excluding one outlier; two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses | ↑ L anterior temporal notes: more activity in patients with better auditory sentence comprehension |
Crinion et al. (2006): ROI 2 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia with no temporal damage (excluding 1 with missing behavioral data and 1 outlier) or posterior temporal damage sparing anterior temporal cortex (n = 13) Covariate: time post onset |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ATL; how ROI defined: activation in the control group; two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses | None |
Crinion et al. (2006): ROI 3 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia with temporal damage excluding anterior temporal cortex (n = 9) vs with no temporal lobe damage (excluding 1 with missing behavioral data and 1 outlier) (n = 4) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ATL; how ROI defined: activation in the control group; two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses | ↓ L anterior temporal notes: patients with posterior temporal damage had less signal change |
Crinion et al. (2006): ROI 4 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia with temporal damage excluding anterior temporal cortex (n = 9) vs control |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ATL; how ROI defined: activation in the control group; circular because ROI defined in one group; two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses | ↓ L anterior temporal notes: large difference 2.7 ± 0.8 (patients) vs 6.3 ± 1.4 (controls) makes finding suggestive even in light of the circularity |
Crinion et al. (2006): ROI 5 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia with no temporal damage (excluding 1 with missing behavioral data and 1 outlier) or posterior temporal damage sparing anterior temporal cortex (n = 13) Covariate: auditory single word comprehension (CAT) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ATL; how ROI defined: activation in the control group; two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses | None notes: r = 0.39; p > 0.1; seems to be a clear trend so lack of significance may reflect only lack of power |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 1 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
AM | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L insula ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal notes: R IFG/insula activation noted to survive FWE correction at p < .05 |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 2 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
AM | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: none; threshold was lowered to reveal the R frontal change in activation | ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R occipital |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 3 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
AM | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R insula |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 4 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L insula ↓ L posterior MTG ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R insula notes: L STG in table is actually MTG based on coordinates |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 5 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia T2 vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: none; threshold was lowered to reveal L IFG | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L insula ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R IFG |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 6 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia T3 vs control |
AS | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | None |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 7 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: language recovery score T1 |
AM | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L IFG ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R IFG pars triangularis |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 8 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: language recovery score T2 |
UNT | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | None |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 9 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T3 Covariate: language recovery score T3 |
UNT | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | None |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 10 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: % change in language recovery score |
UNT | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 11 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T3 vs T2 Covariate: % change in language recovery score |
UNT | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | None |
Saur et al. (2006): Vox 12 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T3 vs T1 Covariate: % change in language recovery score |
UNT | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | None |
Saur et al. (2006): ROI 1 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
AM | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA; how ROIs defined: peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients | ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal notes: some other ROIs also significant prior to correction for multiple comparisons; n.b. performance confound |
Saur et al. (2006): ROI 2 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
AM | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA; how ROIs defined: peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients | None notes: some other ROIs also significant prior to correction for multiple comparisons; n.b. performance confound |
Saur et al. (2006): ROI 3 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
AM | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA; how ROIs defined: peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients | ↑ L posterior MTG notes: some other ROIs also significant prior to correction for multiple comparisons; n.b. performance confound |
Saur et al. (2006): ROI 4 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
AM | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA; how ROIs defined: peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients; circular because ROIs defined in one group | ↓ L posterior MTG ↓ R IFG pars triangularis notes: R IFG difference described in text but not table |
Saur et al. (2006): ROI 5 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia T2 vs control |
AM | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA; how ROIs defined: peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients; circular because ROIs defined in one group | None |
Saur et al. (2006): ROI 6 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia T3 vs control |
AS | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy combines language and control conditions; number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA; how ROIs defined: peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients; circular because ROIs defined in one group | None |
Meinzer et al. (2008): ROI 1 |
Picture naming (trained items) vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ picture naming (trained items) |
C | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: picture naming score (trained items) increased from 51.7 ± 24.8 to 78.8 ± 22.1, which was statistically significant (p < 0.0001); number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) perilesional area of slow wave activity determined with MEG; (2) right hemisphere homotopic to lesion; (3) right hemisphere homotopic to slow wave area; (4) remainder of left hemisphere; for one patient, maximal slow wave activity was in the right hemisphere and it is not clear how this was handled; how ROIs defined: the dependent measure was the number of voxels in each ROI exceeding certain thresholds that differed across subjects depending on their strength of activation; it appears that increases and decreases may have been summed, though the description is hard to follow; 2 of the 11 patients were classified as outliers and excluded from analyses, however no plots are provided to justify their status as outliers | Other: improved picture naming of trained items was correlated with increased signal in 3 of the 4 ROIs, the exception being the right hemisphere ROI homotopic to the slow wave area; after removing the two outliers, only the correlation in the left hemisphere area of slow wave activity remained significant |
Meinzer et al. (2008): ROI 2 |
Picture naming (untrained items) vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ picture naming (untrained items) |
C | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: picture naming score (untrained items) increased from 54.0 ± 24.3 to 70.5 ± 26.7, which was statistically significant (p= 0.002); number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) perilesional area of slow wave activity determined with MEG; (2) right hemisphere homotopic to lesion; (3) right hemisphere homotopic to slow wave area; (4) remainder of left hemisphere; for one patient, maximal slow wave activity was in the right hemisphere and it is not clear how this was handled; how ROIs defined: the dependent measure was the number of voxels in each ROI exceeding certain thresholds that differed across subjects depending on their strength of activation; it appears that increases and decreases may have been summed, though the description is hard to follow; 2 of the 11 patients were classified as outliers and excluded from analyses, however no plots are provided to justify their status as outliers | Other: improved picture naming of untrained items was correlated with increased signal in all 4 ROIs; after removing the two outliers, none of the correlations remained significant |
Raboyeau et al. (2008): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (native in patients; relearned foreign in controls) vs rest | LAC (Aphasia T2 vs T1) vs (control T2 vs T1) |
NAM | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: relearned foreign language was an attempt to equate to recovery in patients; still, patients improved less than controls, as shown by a significant interaction of group by time (p < .0001); search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 30 voxels (size not stated); nature of control contrast not clear; negative tail of contrast was masked to exclude lesioned areas, but the mask may have been more extensive than that | ↑ L orbitofrontal |
Raboyeau et al. (2008): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (native in patients; relearned foreign in controls) vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ picture naming accuracy |
C | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 30 voxels (size not stated); nature of control contrast not clear | ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L intraparietal sulcus ↓ L precuneus ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ R dorsal precentral ↓ R precuneus |
Richter et al. (2008): Vox 1 |
Reading words silently vs rest | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox M** |
Search volume: R hemisphere; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.7; voxelwise p: R IFG/R insula ROI: .005; elsewhere: .001; cluster extent cutoff: R IFG/R insula ROI: 0.108 cc; elsewhere: none | ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula |
Richter et al. (2008): Vox 2 |
Word stem completion vs rest | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox M** |
Search volume: R hemisphere; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.7; voxelwise p: R IFG/R insula ROI: .005; elsewhere: .001; cluster extent cutoff: R IFG/R insula ROI: 0.108 cc; elsewhere: none | ↑ R dorsal precentral |
Richter et al. (2008): Vox 3 |
Reading words silently vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: R hemisphere; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.7; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: none; nature of thresholding not entirely clear, so coded according to best guess | ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R posterior MTG notes: increased activity correlated with more behavioral improvement |
Richter et al. (2008): Vox 4 |
Word stem completion vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: R hemisphere; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.7; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: none; nature of thresholding not entirely clear, so coded according to best guess | ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula notes: increased activity correlated with more behavioral improvement |
Richter et al. (2008): Vox 5 |
Reading words silently vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox M** |
Search volume: R hemisphere; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.7; voxelwise p: R IFG/R insula ROI: .005; elsewhere: .001; cluster extent cutoff: R IFG/R insula ROI: 0.108 cc; elsewhere: none | None |
Richter et al. (2008): Vox 6 |
Word stem completion vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox M** |
Search volume: R hemisphere; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.7; voxelwise p: R IFG/R insula ROI: .005; elsewhere: .001; cluster extent cutoff: R IFG/R insula ROI: 0.108 cc; elsewhere: none | None |
Richter et al. (2008): ROI 1 |
Reading words silently vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L IFG/insula or L perilesional; how ROI defined: peak activations in individual patients in L IFG/insula or L perilesional regions (somewhat unclear) | None |
Richter et al. (2008): ROI 2 |
Word stem completion vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L IFG/insula or L perilesional; how ROI defined: peak activations in individual patients in L IFG/insula or L perilesional regions (somewhat unclear) | None |
Richter et al. (2008): ROI 3 |
Reading words silently vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) R IFG/insula; (2) R precentral; (3) R MTG; (4) L IFG/insula or L perilesional; how ROIs defined: regions where T1 activation was correlated with subsequent improvement, along with the previously defined left hemisphere ROI; circular because functional ROIs based on related contrast on same data | ↓ R posterior MTG notes: decreased activity over time correlated with more behavioral improvement |
Richter et al. (2008): ROI 4 |
Word stem completion vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1, 2) two clusters within R IFG/insula ROI; (3) L IFG/insula or L perilesional; how ROIs defined: regions where T1 activation was correlated with subsequent improvement, along with the previously defined left hemisphere ROI; circular because functional ROIs based on related contrast on same data | ↓ R IFG ↓ R insula notes: decreased activity over time correlated with more behavioral improvement |
de Boissezon et al. (2009): Vox 1 |
Word generation vs rest | LA Aphasia with "good recovery" (n = 6) T2 vs T1 Somewhat valid (the "good recovery" group showed more improvement than the "poor recovery" group in terms of accuracy on the task, but the distinction was not borne out in behavioral data more generally) |
Y | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: p = 0.07; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 100 voxels (size not stated); contrast may not have included resting condition; inappropriate masking | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R occipital ↑ R thalamus ↑ R basal ganglia ↓ L cerebellum notes: based on coordinates in Table 5 |
de Boissezon et al. (2009): Vox 2 |
Word generation vs rest | LA Aphasia with "poor recovery" (n = 7) T2 vs T1 Somewhat valid (the "poor recovery" group showed less improvement than the "good recovery" group in terms of accuracy on the task, but the distinction was not borne out in behavioral data more generally) |
Y | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 100 voxels (size not stated); contrast may not have included resting condition; inappropriate masking | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R cerebellum ↓ R basal ganglia |
de Boissezon et al. (2009): Vox 3 |
Word generation vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: word generation accuracy |
C | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 100 voxels (size not stated); each patient's two sessions may be entered into the model without accounting for the dependence between them | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital ↓ L cerebellum |
Fridriksson et al. (2009): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CAC Aphasia vs control |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: FSL (FEAT 5.4); voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | None |
Fridriksson et al. (2009): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (phonemic paraphasias) vs picture naming (correct trials) | CB Aphasia |
NBD | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: FSL (FEAT 5.4); voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L occipital |
Fridriksson et al. (2009): Vox 3 |
Picture naming (semantic paraphasias) vs picture naming (correct trials) | CB Aphasia |
NBD | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: FSL (FEAT 5.4); voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R occipital |
Fridriksson et al. (2009): ROI 1 |
Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia Covariate: picture naming accuracy |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) R IFG/insula; (2) R motor/premotor; (3) R SMA; (4) R inferior parietal; (5) R superior temporal; how ROIs defined: regions activated for picture naming vs viewing scrambled images in aphasia | ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula notes: R IFG showed more activation in patients who produced more correct responses |
Menke et al. (2009): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (trained items) vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: subsequent outcome (T2) picture naming of trained items outside the scanner Not valid (the logic behind correlating activation changes and language outcome is unclear) |
UNT | UNR | Vox M** |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .05, but at least one voxel in the cluster had to be p < .001; cluster extent cutoff: 0.270 cc; there was an exclusive mask based on activation changes for untrained pictures, but it is unclear what the behavioral covariate was for the mask generation, nor were the regions in the mask reported | ↑ L occipital ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R precuneus ↑ R occipital ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R hippocampus/MTL |
Menke et al. (2009): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (untrained items) vs rest | LC Aphasia T3 vs T1 Covariate: subsequent outcome (T3) picture naming of trained items outside the scanner Not valid (the logic behind correlating activation changes and language outcome is unclear) |
UNT | UNR | Vox M** |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; voxelwise p: .05, but at least one voxel in the cluster had to be p < .001; cluster extent cutoff: 0.270 cc; there was an exclusive mask based on activation changes for untrained pictures, but it is unclear what the behavioral covariate was for the mask generation, nor were the regions in the mask reported | ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R inferior parietal lobule ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R basal ganglia |
Specht et al. (2009): Vox 1 |
Lexical decision (words vs pseudowords) vs lexical decision (words vs reversed foreign words) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM5; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 0.64 cc | ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R Heschl's gyrus notes: activation is 1105 voxels (> 8 cc) so quite convincing, but when the contrast was examined in the patient group, this region was not activated. |
Specht et al. (2009): Cplx 1 |
Lexical decision (words vs pseudowords) vs lexical decision (words vs reversed foreign words) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Joint ICA was performed on structural and functional contrast images using FIT 1.1b. Only 1 of the 8 components differed between groups in its loadings and was interpretable. The structural part of this component related to the patients' lesions. The functional part was thresholded at voxelwise p < .001 (CDT), arbitrary minimum cluster extent = 0.64 cc. | Other: The component that differed between groups showed more activation for patients than controls in the L anterior temporal lobe, L cerebellum, R posterior STG, R anterior temporal lobe, R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus, R cerebellum, and R brainstem, and less activation in patients than controls in the L IFG, L anterior temporal lobe, L occipital lobe, L anterior cingulate, L cerebellum, L thalamus, and R IFG. |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 1 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6); somewhat circular because ROIs were defined only in regions where controls showed significant connectivity (even though ROIs were anatomical) | None notes: L IFG pars triangularis almost reached significance (p = .053) for more activation in patients |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 2 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia Covariate: auditory sentence comprehension |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) | ↑ L anterior temporal |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 3 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia Covariate: written sentence comprehension |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) | None |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 4 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia Covariate: auditory single word comprehension |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) | None notes: L anterior temporal p = .08 |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 5 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia Covariate: auditory syntactic comprehension |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) | None notes: L anterior temporal p = .09 |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 6 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia Covariate: connectivity between L and R ATL |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) R anterior superior temporal cortex; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) | None |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 7 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia Covariate: time post onset |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L anterior superior temporal cortex; how ROI defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) | None |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 8 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L anterior superior temporal cortex; how ROI defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) | None |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 9 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia with positive anterior temporal interconnectivity (n = 8) vs control |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6); somewhat circular because ROIs were defined only in regions where controls showed significant connectivity (even though ROIs were anatomical); excluded 3 patients with L IFG damage | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 10 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia with negative anterior temporal interconnectivity (n = 8) vs control |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6); somewhat circular because ROIs were defined only in regions where controls showed significant connectivity (even though ROIs were anatomical); excluded 1 patient with L IFG damage | None |
Warren et al. (2009): ROI 11 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia with positive anterior temporal interconnectivity (n = 8) vs with negative anterior temporal interconnectivity (n = 8) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6); excluded 4 patients with L IFG damage | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis |
Warren et al. (2009): Cplx 1 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion status of each voxel |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
VLSM with FDR correction was used to identify any regions in which damage was predictive of L anterior temporal activation. | None |
Chau et al. (2010): Vox 1 |
Answering questions from Cantonese Aphasia Battery vs visual decision | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ WAB AQ Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
UNR | UNR | Vox U |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM2; stated to be corrected p < 0.05, but the nature of correction is not described; it is not entirely clear whether the functional measure was the difference between T1 and T2 (we assume it is); it is also not clear whether or not 2 patients with low AQ were excluded (we assume not) | ↑ L posterior MTG notes: finding based on table; additional small activations are shown in figure but not table |
Fridriksson (2010): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ picture naming accuracy |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 4.1; voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L precuneus notes: activated regions were on the borders on the lesion distribution in the 19 included patients |
Fridriksson et al. (2010): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures | CC Aphasia Covariate: picture naming accuracy |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 4.1; voxelwise p: ~.02 (z > 2); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate notes: greater activation was associated with better picture naming; L IFG pars orbitalis activation classified as middle frontal gyrus in the paper, but coordinates suggest otherwise |
Fridriksson et al. (2010): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures | CAC Aphasia vs control |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 4.1; voxelwise p: ~.02 (z > 2); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | None |
Fridriksson et al. (2010): ROI 1 |
Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures | CC Aphasia Covariate: picture naming accuracy |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: a single ROI comprising 3 regions where activation in patients was correlated with picture naming accuracy: the L IFG pars orbitalis, occipital lobe, and anterior cingulate; how ROI defined: based on SPM analysis 1; the purpose of this analysis was to determine whether these regions were recruited in the patients with better naming, or not activated in the patients with worse naming, relative to the control mean | Other: patients with better naming showed greater activation than controls, while the patients with poorer naming showed less activation than controls. |
Fridriksson et al. (2010): Cplx 1 |
Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion status of each voxel |
YCT | UNR | Cplx |
VLSM was used to identify any regions in which damage was predictive of activation in the regions identified in SPM analysis 1, considered as a single ROI. There was no correction for multiple comparisons, and the analysis is appropriately presented as exploratory. | Other: Only in the L IFG pars opercularis was damage predictive of reduced activation in the potentially compensatory network. |
Sharp et al. (2010): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision (clear in patients; average of clear and noise vocoded in controls) vs syllable count decision (clear in patients; average of clear and noise vocoded in controls) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NAM | AS | ROI Oth NDC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy and RT were not significantly different for the semantic task; statistics are not reported for the syllable counting task, but the data provided suggest that accuracy was probably not matched, while RT probably was; number of ROIs: 12; ROIs: functional connectivity between pairs of spared nodes of the L hemisphere semantic network and R hemisphere homotopic regions: (1) L SFG-L AG; (2) L SFG-L IFG; (3) L SFG-L IT; (4) L AG-L IFG; (5) L AG-L IT; (6) L IFG-L IT; (7-12) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: partial correlations between nodes | Other: patients showed greater connectivity between L SFG and L AG than controls while performing the semantic task; this was not the case for the syllable counting task, however connectivity during performance of the two tasks was not compared directly |
Thompson et al. (2010): ROI 1 |
Auditory sentence-picture matching (all three sentence types) vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
AS | AS | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 18; ROIs: (1) L BA 7; (2) L BA 9; (3) L BA 13; (4) L BA 21; (5) L BA 22; (6) L BA 39; (7) L BA 40; (8) L BA 44; (9) L BA 45; (10-18) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas; proportion of patients who showed increases and decreases in (parts of) each ROI in individual fixed effects SPM analyses | ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R superior parietal ↓ L insula ↓ L posterior STG notes: these are the regions involved in what the authors interpret as a "general shift" |
Tyler et al. (2010): Vox 1 |
Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | AS | Vox NDC |
Behavioral data notes: the two groups showed similar differences between RTs in the two conditions of the contrast; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM5; qualitative comparison on pp. 3402-3; each group is presented at voxelwise p < .005 (CDT), cluster-corrected p < .05 with GRFT | ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L posterior MTG notes: several other potential differences are apparent in the figure, but only the differences tabulated are interpreted in the text |
Tyler et al. (2010): ROI 1 |
Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word | CC Aphasia Covariate: RT difference between early and late targets on grammatical but meaningless sentences (a measure of syntactic processing) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: analyses focuses on RT differences between early and late targets, not on mean RT per se; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis; how ROI defined: activated for the same contrast | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis notes: L IFG showed more activation in patients that had a larger target position effect (indicative of better syntactic processing) |
Tyler et al. (2010): ROI 2 |
Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word | CC Aphasia Covariate: RT difference between early and late targets on normal sentences |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis; how ROI defined: activated for the same contrast | None |
Tyler et al. (2010): ROI 3 |
Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word | CC Aphasia Covariate: RT difference between early and late targets on scrambled sentences |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis; how ROI defined: activated for the same contrast | None |
Tyler et al. (2010): ROI 4 |
Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word | CC Aphasia Covariate: damage to L IFG, estimated from T1 signal |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis; how ROI defined: activated for the same contrast | None notes: no correlation (p = .57) |
Tyler et al. (2010): ROI 5 |
Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word | CC Aphasia Covariate: syntactic processing (presumably the target position effect, though this is not stated) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis; how ROI defined: activated for the same contrast | None notes: no correlation (p = .41) |
Tyler et al. (2010): Cplx 1 |
Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion status of each voxel |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
VBM was used to identify any regions where damage was predictive of activation in the L IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis. Tissue integrity was quantified in terms of T1 signal. Clusterwise correction was used, which is not appropriate for VBM. | Other: Only in the L IFG itself was damage predictive of reduced activation in the L IFG. |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 1 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy not reported for control condition; number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | ↓ L IFG ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 2 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: accuracy not reported for control condition; number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | ↓ L IFG ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 3 |
Verb generation vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | ↓ L IFG ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 4 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: picture-word matching accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 5 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: semantic decision accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 6 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: overall language measure |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 7 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: overall language measure |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas; not clear if it was LI for whole language network | ↑ LI (language network) |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 8 |
Verb generation vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: overall language measure |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 9 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 10 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 11 |
Verb generation vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 12 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: damage to L hemisphere language regions |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 13 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: damage to L hemisphere language regions |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 14 |
Verb generation vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: damage to L hemisphere language regions |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 15 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: previous (current vs subacute) Δ naming Not valid (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 16 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: previous (current vs subacute) Δ naming Not valid (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | ↑ L IFG |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 17 |
Verb generation vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: previous (current vs subacute) Δ naming Not valid (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | ↑ L IFG |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 18 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: previous (current vs subacute) Δ TT Not valid (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function; TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | None |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 19 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: previous (current vs subacute) Δ TT Not valid (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function; TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | ↑ L IFG ↑ R IFG |
van Oers et al. (2010): ROI 20 |
Verb generation vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: previous (current vs subacute) Δ TT Not valid (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function; TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 7; ROIs: (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI; how ROIs defined: WFU pickatlas | ↑ L IFG ↑ R IFG |
Papoutsi et al. (2011): Vox 1 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") | CC Aphasia Covariate: difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between subordinate and dominant sentences (dominance effect) |
NANB | NANT | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L insula ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ L mid temporal |
Papoutsi et al. (2011): Cplx 1 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") | CC Aphasia Covariate: modulation of L IFG connectivity by dominance effect |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
A PPI analysis was carried out with the L IFG as the seed region. Correlations were computed between voxelwise modulation of connectivity with this region, and a behavioral measure of syntactic processing, which was the dominance effect: the difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between subordinate and dominant sentences. The resultant SPM was thresholded at voxelwise p < .01 (CDT), then corrected for multiple corrections based on cluster extent and GRFT using SPM8. | Other: patients with better syntactic performance had more connectivity from the L IFG seed region to L pMTG and adjacent areas (including the insula); pMTG also significant at voxelwise p < .001 in Figure 2B, corrected for multiple comparisons with GRFT |
Papoutsi et al. (2011): Cplx 2 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") | CC Aphasia Covariate: modulation of L pMTG connectivity by dominance effect |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
A similar PPI analysis was carried out with the L pMTG as the seed region. Thresholding was the same as in the previous analysis. | None |
Sebastian & Kiran (2011): ROI 1 |
Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "pass" | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
YCT | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L IFG (oper/tri); (2) L posterior perisylvian (pSTG, pMTG, AG, SMG); (3) R IFG (oper/tri); (4) R posterior perisylvian (pSTG, pMTG, AG, SMG); (5) language network LI; how ROIs defined: Harvard–Oxford atlas | ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ LI (language network) notes: larger lesions were associated with more R posterior perisylvian activation |
Sebastian & Kiran (2011): ROI 2 |
Semantic decision (correct trials) vs visual decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
YCT | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L IFG (oper/tri); (2) L posterior perisylvian (pSTG, pMTG, AG, SMG); (3) R IFG (oper/tri); (4) R posterior perisylvian (pSTG, pMTG, AG, SMG); (5) language network LI; how ROIs defined: Harvard–Oxford atlas | None |
Szaflarski et al. (2011): Vox 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 Somewhat valid (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Y | UNR | Vox NC |
Behavioral data notes: language and control tasks both matched; search volume: whole brain; software: in-house; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: none; the figure shows a cutoff of z > 10, which would not correspond to p < .05; increases and decreases in Figure 3 do not accord with the data from T1 and T2 in Figure 2, raising concerns about the implementation of the analyses; there is no explicit description of the second level analysis | ↑ L IFG ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R precuneus ↑ R occipital ↑ R basal ganglia ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ R insula ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R posterior STG notes: based on a combination of coordinates in Table 2, and Figure 3 |
Szaflarski et al. (2011): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 Somewhat valid (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Y | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: language and control tasks both matched; number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI; (3) language network LI; T1 LI (temporal) is reported to be negative, which does not accord with the voxelwise analysis in Figure 2; increases and decreases in Figure 3 do not accord with the data from T1 and T2 in Figure 2, raising concerns about the implementation of the analyses | ↑ LI (language network) ↑ LI (frontal) ↑ LI (temporal) |
Tyler et al. (2011): Vox 1 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox NDC |
Search volume: plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions; software: SPM5; qualitative comparison on p. 423 | ↓ L IFG |
Tyler et al. (2011): Vox 2 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox NDC |
Search volume: plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions; software: SPM5; qualitative comparison on p. 423 | ↓ L IFG |
Tyler et al. (2011): Vox 3 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox NDC |
Search volume: plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions; software: SPM5; qualitative comparison on p. 423 | ↓ L IFG notes: lack of patient activation in pMTG implied in text, but this activation looks fairly similar in patients and controls (c.f. Figure 3C vs 2C) |
Tyler et al. (2011): Vox 4 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NANB | NANT | Vox NDC |
Search volume: plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions; software: SPM5; qualitative comparison on p. 423 | ↓ L IFG ↓ L posterior MTG |
Tyler et al. (2011): Vox 5 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CC Aphasia Covariate: performance on acceptability judgment task (difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between ambiguous and unambiguous sentences) |
NANB | NANT | Vox C- |
Search volume: plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions; software: SPM5; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R insula ↑ R mid temporal notes: also L pMTG but this did not reach significance |
Tyler et al. (2011): Vox 6 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CC Aphasia Covariate: performance on sentence-picture matching task |
NANB | NANT | Vox CA |
Search volume: plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions; software: SPM5; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 30 (units not stated) | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ R insula ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R mid temporal |
Tyler et al. (2011): Vox 7 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CC Aphasia Covariate: performance on word monitoring task |
NANB | NANT | Vox CA |
Search volume: plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions; software: SPM5; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 10 (units not stated) | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ R insula ↑ R mid temporal |
Tyler et al. (2011): Vox 8 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CC Aphasia Covariate: difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between subordinate and dominant sentences (dominance effect) |
NANB | NANT | Vox C- |
Search volume: plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions; software: SPM5; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | None |
Tyler et al. (2011): ROI 1 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CC Aphasia Covariate: performance on acceptability judgment task (difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between ambiguous and unambiguous sentences) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) IFG pars opercularis; (2) IFG pars triangularis; (3) IFG pars orbitalis; how ROIs defined: AAL | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis |
Tyler et al. (2011): ROI 2 |
Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") | CC Aphasia Covariate: difference in percentage of unacceptable judgments between subordinate and dominant sentences (dominance effect) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) IFG pars opercularis; (2) IFG pars triangularis; (3) IFG pars orbitalis; how ROIs defined: AAL | None |
Weiduschat et al. (2011): ROI 1 |
Verb generation vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 (regardless of rTMS) |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) IFG LI; (2) superior temporal LI; (3) SMA LI | None |
Weiduschat et al. (2011): ROI 2 |
Verb generation vs rest | LA Aphasia treated with rTMS (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) IFG LI; (2) superior temporal LI; (3) SMA LI | None |
Weiduschat et al. (2011): ROI 3 |
Verb generation vs rest | LAA (Aphasia with R IFG rTMS (n = 6) T2 vs T1) vs (with sham rTMS (n = 4) T2 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) IFG LI; (2) superior temporal LI; (3) SMA LI | ↑ LI (frontal) notes: IFG LI was stable in the stimulation group, but shifted to the R in the sham group, yielding a significant difference between groups |
Weiduschat et al. (2011): ROI 4 |
Verb generation vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 (regardless of rTMS) Covariate: Δ AAT total score |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: IFG LI | None |
Allendorfer et al. (2012): ROI 1 |
Verb generation (covert, block) vs finger tapping (block) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI | ↓ LI (temporal) |
Allendorfer et al. (2012): ROI 2 |
Verb generation (overt, event-related) vs noun repetition (event-related) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate and produced less responses on both conditions, but the difference between groups was greater for verb generation; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI | ↓ LI (frontal) |
Allendorfer et al. (2012): ROI 3 |
Verb generation (overt, event-related) vs verb generation (covert, event-related) | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Behavioral data notes: overt performance differed, so covert performance probably did too; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI | None notes: lack of lateralization in controls makes this analysis difficult to interpret |
Allendorfer et al. (2012): ROI 4 |
Verb generation (overt, event-related) vs noun repetition (event-related) | CC Aphasia Covariate: overt verb generation accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) L MTG; (2) L SFG/CG; (3) left MFG; how ROIs defined: regions activated by the contrast of overt verb generation vs noun repetition in patients | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Allendorfer et al. (2012): ROI 5 |
Verb generation (overt, event-related) vs verb generation (covert, event-related) | CC Aphasia Covariate: overt verb generation accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) R insula/IFG; (2) R STG; how ROIs defined: prominent R hemisphere activations for the contrast of overt and covert verb generation in patients | None |
Fridriksson, Hubbard, et al. (2012): Vox 1 |
Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences, while producing the same sentences in unison (speech entrainment) vs listening to reversed sentences and viewing a mouth speaking, while producing unrelated sentences | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
UNR | NANT | Vox U |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL (FEAT 5.98); thresholding not stated | ↑ L angular gyrus ↓ L anterior temporal notes: based on coordinates in Table 2 |
Fridriksson, Hubbard, et al. (2012): Vox 2 |
Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences, while producing the same sentences in unison (speech entrainment) vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | NANT | Vox U |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL (FEAT 5.98); thresholding not stated | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R precuneus ↑ R occipital ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ L supramarginal gyrus notes: some labels changed based on coordinates |
Fridriksson, Hubbard, et al. (2012): Vox 3 |
Listening to reversed sentences and viewing a mouth speaking, while producing unrelated sentences vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | NANT | Vox U |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL (FEAT 5.98); thresholding not stated | None |
Fridriksson, Hubbard, et al. (2012): Vox 4 |
Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences and viewing a mouth vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
NANB | NANT | Vox U |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL (FEAT 5.98); thresholding not stated | None |
Fridriksson, Hubbard, et al. (2012): ROI 1 |
Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences, while producing the same sentences in unison (speech entrainment) vs listening to reversed sentences and viewing a mouth speaking, while producing unrelated sentences | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
UNR | NANT | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L anterior insula/IFG pars orbitalis; (2) R anterior insula/IFG pars orbitalis; (3) Broca's area; (4) L MTG; (5) L BA 37; (6) R BA 37; how ROIs defined: regions activated in both groups considered together; there were no interactions of group by condition; two regions showed main effects of group but this is not pertinent to the contrast | None |
Fridriksson, Richardson, et al. (2012): ROI 1 |
Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ picture naming accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions; how ROIs defined: based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming | Other: change in perilesional non-language regions positively correlated with improvement in accuracy |
Fridriksson, Richardson, et al. (2012): ROI 2 |
Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ (decrease in) semantic errors |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions; how ROIs defined: based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming | Other: change in undamaged non-perilesional language regions negatively correlated with decrease in semantic errors |
Fridriksson, Richardson, et al. (2012): ROI 3 |
Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ (decrease in) phonological paraphasias |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions; how ROIs defined: based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming | Other: change in perilesional language regions, and change in undamaged non-perilesional language regions, negatively correlated with decrease in phonological paraphasias |
Fridriksson, Richardson, et al. (2012): ROI 4 |
Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming accuracy Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions; how ROIs defined: based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming | None |
Fridriksson, Richardson, et al. (2012): ROI 5 |
Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1, decrease in) semantic errors Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions; how ROIs defined: based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming | Other: change in perilesional language regions correlated with decrease in phonological paraphasias |
Fridriksson, Richardson, et al. (2012): ROI 6 |
Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1, decrease in) phonological paraphasias Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions; how ROIs defined: based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming | None |
Marcotte et al. (2012): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (T1: known items; T2: trained items; correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
YCT | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM5; qualitative comparison on p. 1780; different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↓ L dorsal precentral ↓ L posterior MTG notes: labels based on figures rather than text |
Marcotte et al. (2012): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (known items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) naming of trained items Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
YCT | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM5; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 10 voxels (size not stated); different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R thalamus notes: labels based on figures and text |
Marcotte et al. (2012): Vox 3 |
Picture naming (trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: previous Δ (T2 vs T1) naming of trained items Not valid (T2 activation not an appropriate measure of treatment-induced recovery because it reflects T2 performance) |
YCT | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM5; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 10 voxels (size not stated); different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained | ↑ L somato-motor notes: label based on figure |
Schofield et al. (2012): Vox 1 |
Listening to word pairs or reversed word pairs, speaker gender judgment vs rest | CAC Moderate aphasia (n = 11) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↓ L Heschl's gyrus notes: structurally, HG was not significantly damaged in this group |
Schofield et al. (2012): Vox 2 |
Listening to word pairs or reversed word pairs, speaker gender judgment vs rest | CAC Severe aphasia (n = 9) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox M** |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: MGB: SVC; elsewhere: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L thalamus notes: specifically: PT, HG and MGB; structurally, the PT and HG were significantly damaged, but not the MGB |
Schofield et al. (2012): Vox 3 |
Listening to word pairs or reversed word pairs, speaker gender judgment vs rest | CAA Severe (n = 9) vs moderate (n = 11) aphasia |
UNR | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↓ L posterior STG notes: specifically, PT; structurally, severe patients had more damage in HG and PT |
Wright et al. (2012): Vox 1 |
Listening to normal sentences and detecting a target word vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
Y | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM5; voxelwise p: .01 | ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal notes: at a more stringent threshold of p < .001, with correction for multiple comparisons based on GRFT and cluster extent, only L HG showed reduced activity in patients |
Wright et al. (2012): Cplx 1 |
Listening to normal sentences and detecting a target word vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: see statistical details |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Joint ICA was performed on structural and functional contrast images for each of the two contrasts using FIT 2.0b. Seven components were derived, of which 2 were further investigated since their loadings correlated with relevant behavioral measures. Functional components were thresholded at p < .001, cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons, minimum cluster extent = 1.27 cc. Component 1 was considered a "semantics component" because it correlated with the semantic behavioral measure and not with either of the two syntactic measures. This component did not have any anatomical aspect to it. Component 2 was considered a "syntax component" because it correlated with both syntactic behavioral measures and not with the semantic measure. This conceptualization seems somewhat speculative, given that WPE NP and WPE AP are rather indirect measures of syntactic and semantic processing. Component 2 involved damage to left frontal and insular cortex, and underlying dorsal white matter. | Other: Contrast 1 loaded primarily on the R STG for component 1 (the "semantics component") and on the L ITG for component 2 (the "syntax component"). |
Wright et al. (2012): Cplx 2 |
Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: see statistical details |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Joint ICA was performed on structural and functional contrast images for each of the two contrasts using FIT 2.0b. Seven components were derived, of which 2 were further investigated since their loadings correlated with relevant behavioral measures. Functional components were thresholded at p < .001, cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons, minimum cluster extent = 1.27 cc. Component 1 was considered a "semantics component" because it correlated with the semantic behavioral measure and not with either of the two syntactic measures. This component did not have any anatomical aspect to it. Component 2 was considered a "syntax component" because it correlated with both syntactic behavioral measures and not with the semantic measure. This conceptualization seems somewhat speculative, given that WPE NP and WPE AP are rather indirect measures of syntactic and semantic processing. Component 2 involved damage to left frontal and insular cortex, and underlying dorsal white matter. | Other: Contrast 2 loaded primarily on the R posterior STG for component 1 (the "semantics component") and on the L posterior STG and L IFG for component 2 (the "syntax component"). |
Szaflarski et al. (2013): Vox 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAA Aphasia not recovered (n = 18) vs recovered (n = 9) |
AM | UNR | Vox CCS |
Behavioral data notes: interaction of group by condition not reported; non-recovered patients were significantly less accurate only on the semantic decision condition, but they actually showed a smaller difference between conditions than the recovered patients; search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 4.16 cc; cluster-defining threshold (CDT) p < 0.05 too lenient | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R cerebellum ↓ R posterior STG |
Szaflarski et al. (2013): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) Covariate: BNT |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus; how ROIs defined: regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs; circular because defined based on recovered status | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Szaflarski et al. (2013): ROI 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) Covariate: semantic fluency |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus; how ROIs defined: regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs; circular because defined based on recovered status | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Szaflarski et al. (2013): ROI 3 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) Covariate: single word comprehension (PPVT) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus; how ROIs defined: regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs; circular because defined based on recovered status | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Szaflarski et al. (2013): ROI 4 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) Covariate: BDAE complex ideation subtest |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus; how ROIs defined: regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs; circular because defined based on recovered status | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Szaflarski et al. (2013): ROI 5 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) Covariate: phonemic fluency |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus; how ROIs defined: regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs; circular because defined based on recovered status | ↓ R posterior STG |
Szaflarski et al. (2013): ROI 6 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) Covariate: semantic decision accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus; how ROIs defined: regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs; circular because defined based on recovered status | None |
Thiel et al. (2013): Vox 1 |
Verb generation vs rest | LAA (Aphasia with rTMS (n = 13) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with sham (n = 11) T2 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; qualitative comparison on p. 2244 | ↑ L IFG ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ R IFG ↓ R posterior STG/STS/MTG notes: approximate interpretation of qualitative patterns shown in Figure 3; T1 R lateralization surprising relative to other findings from this group |
Thiel et al. (2013): ROI 1 |
Verb generation vs rest | LAA (Aphasia with rTMS (n = 13) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with sham (n = 11) T2 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: language network LI; actual LIs are not reported, only change in LI | ↑ LI (language network) notes: T1 R lateralization surprising relative to other findings from this group |
Thiel et al. (2013): ROI 2 |
Verb generation vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ AAT total score |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: language network LI; model did not include treatment group (rTMS vs sham) | ↑ LI (language network) notes: patients who improved more showed a greater leftward shift of activation; T1 R lateralization surprising relative to other findings from this group |
Abel et al. (2014): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
C | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ R basal ganglia |
Abel et al. (2014): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ picture naming accuracy |
C | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L posterior STS ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ L occipital |
Abel et al. (2014): Vox 3 |
Picture naming (trained items) vs picture naming (untrained items) | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
N | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Behavioral data notes: trained items improved more than untrained items; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L thalamus ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R thalamus ↑ R basal ganglia |
Abel et al. (2014): Vox 4 |
Picture naming (semantic trained items) vs picture naming (phonological trained items) | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Y | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Behavioral data notes: no differential effects for semantic vs phonological trained items; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ R superior parietal ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L occipital ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ R precuneus ↓ R occipital ↓ R anterior cingulate ↓ R posterior cingulate ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Abel et al. (2014): Vox 5 |
Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with semantic impairment T1 (n = 8) vs with phonological impairment T1 (n = 6) |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Abel et al. (2014): Vox 6 |
Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest | LAA (Aphasia with semantic impairment (n = 8) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with phonological impairment (n = 6) T2 vs T1) |
N | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Behavioral data notes: phonological patients showed more improvement on trained items; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L occipital ↑ L thalamus ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ R somato-motor ↓ L IFG pars opercularis |
Abel et al. (2014): Vox 7 |
Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest | LA Aphasia with semantic impairment (n = 8) T2 vs T1 |
N | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L basal ganglia |
Abel et al. (2014): Vox 8 |
Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest | LA Aphasia with phonological impairment (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
N | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | None |
Benjamin et al. (2014): ROI 1 |
Word generation vs rest | LA Aphasia with intention treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI | ↓ LI (frontal) notes: laterality shift for lateral frontal LI, not medial frontal LI |
Benjamin et al. (2014): ROI 2 |
Word generation vs rest | LA Aphasia with intention treatment (n = 6) T3 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI | ↓ LI (frontal) notes: laterality shift for both lateral and medial frontal LIs |
Benjamin et al. (2014): ROI 3 |
Word generation vs rest | LA Aphasia with control treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI | None |
Benjamin et al. (2014): ROI 4 |
Word generation vs rest | LA Aphasia with control treatment (n = 7) T3 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI | None |
Benjamin et al. (2014): ROI 5 |
Word generation vs rest | LC Aphasia with intention treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ category-member generation probe performance |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI | ↓ LI (temporal) |
Benjamin et al. (2014): ROI 6 |
Word generation vs rest | LC Aphasia with control treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ category-member generation probe performance |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI | None |
Benjamin et al. (2014): ROI 7 |
Word generation vs rest | LC Aphasia with intention treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ picture naming probe performance |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI | None |
Benjamin et al. (2014): ROI 8 |
Word generation vs rest | LC Aphasia with control treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ picture naming probe performance |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI | None |
Brownsett et al. (2014): Vox 1 |
Listening to sentences vs listening to segmented white noise | CAC Aphasia (T2 and T3) vs control (T1 and T2) |
N | NANT | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: significant difference in accuracy of subsequent repetition; search volume: whole brain; software: FSL (FEAT 5.98); voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L precuneus ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R precuneus ↓ R posterior cingulate notes: findings are approximate since description is partially in terms of networks; at the earlier time point only, patients also showed reduced activity in left ventral prefrontal cortex and right medial planum temporale |
Brownsett et al. (2014): Vox 2 |
Listening to sentences (patients) or listening to noise vocoded sentences (controls) vs listening to segmented white noise | CAC Aphasia (T2 and T3) vs control (T1 and T2) |
Y | NANT | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: no significant difference in accuracy of subsequent repetition; search volume: whole brain; software: FSL (FEAT 5.98); voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | None |
Brownsett et al. (2014): ROI 1 |
Listening to sentences vs listening to segmented white noise | CC Aphasia mean of T1, T2, T3 Covariate: picture description score (CAT), mean of T1, T2, T3 |
UNR | NANT | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: referring to accuracy of subsequent repetition; correlation with picture description is not reported; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: dorsal anterior cingulate cortex/midline superior frontal gyrus; how ROI defined: contrast of listening to vocoded speech and listening to normal speech in controls; same result obtained with age and lesion volume included in the model | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R anterior cingulate notes: increased activation of dACC/SFG was correlated with higher scores on picture description |
Mattioli et al. (2014): Vox 1 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia treated T2 (n = 6) vs untreated T2 (n = 6) Somewhat valid (groups were different but not due to treatment) |
Y | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.9; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 0.16 cc; methods report cluster extent threshold (we assume this was done), but figure caption states uncorrected | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R supramarginal gyrus notes: based on coordinates in Table 2 |
Mattioli et al. (2014): Vox 2 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia treated T3 (n = 6) vs untreated T3 (n = 6) Somewhat valid (groups were different but not due to treatment) |
Y | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.9; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 0.16 cc; methods report cluster extent threshold (we assume this was done), but figure caption states uncorrected | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L insula ↑ L supramarginal gyrus notes: based on coordinates in Table 2; also increases in R IFG and R supramarginal gyrus but only uncorrected |
Mattioli et al. (2014): Vox 3 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia treated (n = 6) T2 vs T1) vs (untreated (n = 6) T2 vs T1) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
Y | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.9; qualitative comparison on p. 548 | ↑ L IFG ↑ R posterior STG ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ R IFG notes: treated patients showed increases in L IFG and R STG, while untreated patients showed increases in L IPL and R IFG |
Mattioli et al. (2014): Vox 4 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia treated (n = 6) T3 vs T2) vs (untreated (n = 6) T3 vs T2) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
Y | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.9; qualitative comparison on p. 548 | None notes: the two groups were reported to have comparable increases in L hemisphere language areas |
Mattioli et al. (2014): Vox 5 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia treated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Y | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.9; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R posterior STG |
Mattioli et al. (2014): Vox 6 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia untreated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Y | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.9; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ R insula |
Mattioli et al. (2014): Vox 7 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia treated (n = 6) T3 vs T2 |
Y | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.9; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L IFG ↑ L insula ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ R insula |
Mattioli et al. (2014): Vox 8 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia untreated (n = 6) T3 vs T2 |
Y | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: BrainVoyager QX 1.9; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R angular gyrus |
Mattioli et al. (2014): ROI 1 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia treated (n = 6) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) vs (untreated (n = 6) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
Y | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG; how ROIs defined: based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used | ↑ L IFG notes: interaction of time by treatment: treated group showed greater L IFG activity at T2 |
Mattioli et al. (2014): ROI 2 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia treated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ written language (AAT) |
Y | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG; how ROIs defined: based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used | None |
Mattioli et al. (2014): ROI 3 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia treated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ naming (AAT) |
Y | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG; how ROIs defined: based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used | ↑ L IFG |
Mattioli et al. (2014): ROI 4 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia untreated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ written language (AAT) |
Y | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG; how ROIs defined: based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used | None |
Mattioli et al. (2014): ROI 5 |
Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia untreated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ naming (AAT) |
Y | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG; how ROIs defined: based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used | ↑ R IFG |
Mohr et al. (2014): Vox 1 |
Listening to sentences (high and low ambiguity) vs listening to signal-correlated noise | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
NANB | NANT | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; qualitative generalization across individuals on pp. 8-9 | None |
Mohr et al. (2014): ROI 1 |
Listening to high ambiguity sentences vs listening to low ambiguity sentences | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L ITG; (4) R ITG; the temporal ROIs are described as STG but they seem to be in the ITG; how ROIs defined: defined based on control data from Rodd et al. (2005) but the coordinates do not match so it is not clear exactly how they were defined; ANOVA of timepoint by hemisphere by site, with a significant interaction of timepoint by hemisphere | ↑ R IFG ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus notes: all signal changes were negative (i.e. less activation for ambiguous sentences), making interpretation challenging |
Robson et al. (2014): Vox 1 |
Semantic decision (written word and picture) vs visual decision and rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | N | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: patients also less accurate on control condition, but control condition includes rest so coded based on language condition only; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 4 voxels (size not stated); dual baseline computation not explained | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L cerebellum ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R mid temporal ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ R posterior cingulate |
Robson et al. (2014): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision (written word and picture) vs visual decision and rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | N | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients also less accurate on control condition, but control condition includes rest so coded based on language condition only; number of ROIs: 10; ROIs: (1) L anterior fusiform gyrus; (2) L temporal pole; (3) L anterior STS; (4) L IFG; (5) L ventral occipito-temporal; (6-10) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: spheres around functional peaks from literature; dual baseline computation not explained | ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Szaflarski et al. (2014): Vox 1 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: CCHIPS; qualitative comparison on pp. 5-6 (page numbers refer to PMC author manuscript) | ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L occipital ↓ R occipital |
Szaflarski et al. (2014): ROI 1 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI; (3) language network LI | ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) notes: temporal LI was also marginally significantly reduced (p = .08) |
van Hees et al. (2014): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming (phonological treated items) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
YCT | UNR | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 0.999 cc | None |
van Hees et al. (2014): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming (semantic treated items) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
YCT | UNR | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 0.999 cc | ↑ L basal ganglia |
van Hees et al. (2014): Vox 3 |
Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: previous Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming (phonological treated items) Not valid (T2 activation not an appropriate measure of treatment-induced recovery because it reflects T2 performance) |
YCT | UNR | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 0.999 cc | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ R precuneus |
van Hees et al. (2014): Vox 4 |
Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: previous Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming (semantic treated items) Not valid (T2 activation not an appropriate measure of treatment-induced recovery because it reflects T2 performance) |
YCT | UNR | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 0.999 cc | None |
van Hees et al. (2014): Vox 5 |
Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent outcome (T2) picture naming Not valid (not appropriate to correlate T1 imaging with T2 behavior without T1 behavior in model) |
YCT | UNR | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 0.999 cc | None |
van Hees et al. (2014): Vox 6 |
Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent outcome (T2) picture naming Not valid (not appropriate to correlate T1 imaging with T2 behavior without T1 behavior in model) |
YCT | UNR | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 0.999 cc | None |
van Hees et al. (2014): Vox 7 |
Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: picture naming T2 |
YCT | UNR | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 0.999 cc | None |
van Hees et al. (2014): Vox 8 |
Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: picture naming T2 |
YCT | UNR | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .005; cluster extent cutoff: 0.999 cc | None |
Abel et al. (2015): Vox 1 |
Picture naming vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
N | N | Vox CCTB |
Behavioral data notes: RT shorter at T2; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L dorsal precentral ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ L precuneus ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R somato-motor ↓ R precuneus ↓ R posterior STS ↓ R posterior MTG ↓ R posterior cingulate ↓ R cerebellum ↓ R thalamus ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Abel et al. (2015): Vox 2 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control T1 |
AM | N | Vox CCTB |
Behavioral data notes: controls responded more quickly; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ R precuneus ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L thalamus ↓ L basal ganglia ↓ R insula ↓ R somato-motor ↓ R mid temporal |
Abel et al. (2015): Vox 3 |
Picture naming vs rest | LAC (Aphasia T2 vs T1) vs (control T2 vs T1) |
AM | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Behavioral data notes: RT not reported for controls; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 11 voxels (size not stated) | ↓ L precuneus ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L basal ganglia ↓ R precuneus ↓ R posterior STS ↓ R posterior MTG ↓ R posterior cingulate ↓ R thalamus ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Abel et al. (2015): Vox 4 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control T1 |
AM | UNR | Vox NDC |
Behavioral data notes: RT not reported for controls; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; qualitative comparison between activation in the first 5 TRs after each stimulus on p. 1101 | None notes: the time course of response is stated to be similar in patients and controls, however the response in patients appears like it could be a couple of seconds slower |
Abel et al. (2015): Cplx 1 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: RT not reported for controls; Joint ICA was performed on structural and functional contrast images using FIT 1.2c. Three of the 7 components differed between groups in their loadings. Components were thresholded at z > 3.09, not corrected for multiple comparisons. | Other: Three structural-functional components are described in Figure 5 and Table 4. Functional activations are generally small and do not obviously relate to language processing. It is mentioned in the supplementary results that "the lesion maps may dominate estimation of the mixing parameter" (p. 10). |
Kiran et al. (2015): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (trained) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "skip" | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; analyses were carried out in individual patients at p < .001, uncorrected; regions were considered activated when they were found in 6 or more (out of 8) patients | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R posterior MTG ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus notes: regions are approximate since only broad regions are described in Table 6 |
Kiran et al. (2015): Vox 2 |
Semantic feature decision vs visual decision | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; analyses were carried out in individual patients at p < .001, uncorrected; regions were considered activated when they were found in 6 or more (out of 8) patients | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R posterior MTG notes: regions are approximate since only broad regions are described in Table 7 |
Sandberg et al. (2015): Vox 1 |
Concreteness judgment (abstract words, correct trials) vs rest | LA Aphasia with response to treatment (n = 9) T2 vs T1 |
Y | Y | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none; images show peaks instead of activations | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital |
Sandberg et al. (2015): Vox 2 |
Concreteness judgment (concrete words, correct trials) vs rest | LA Aphasia with generalization of treatment effects to concrete words (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Y | Y | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none; images show peaks instead of activations | ↑ L insula ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L occipital ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R posterior cingulate |
Geranmayeh et al. (2016): ROI 1 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: difference in AICW/trial; number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network; how ROIs defined: identified using ICA in controls; circular because ROIs defined in one group | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate |
Geranmayeh et al. (2016): ROI 2 |
Propositional speech production vs counting | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: difference in AICW/trial; number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network; how ROIs defined: identified using ICA in controls; circular because ROIs defined in one group | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L IFG ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Geranmayeh et al. (2016): ROI 3 |
Propositional speech production vs target decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: difference in AICW/trial; number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network; how ROIs defined: identified using ICA in controls; circular because ROIs defined in one group | None |
Geranmayeh et al. (2016): Cplx 1 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: difference in AICW/trial; Activity was compared between pairs of ICA-derived networks. However, circularity was introduced because the networks were defined based on the control group. | Other: Patients showed greater differential activation than controls between (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (3) cingulo-opercular network and the DMN. |
Geranmayeh et al. (2016): Cplx 2 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: appropriate information-carrying words |
C | UNR | Cplx |
Multiple regression was used to determine whether differential activation between networks was predictive of the behavioral measure: appropriate information-carrying words. There is no issue of circularity with this analysis since it involved only individuals with aphasia. | Other: Differential activation between L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN was positively correlated with AICW. Differential activation between R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN was negatively correlated with AICW. |
Geranmayeh et al. (2016): Cplx 3 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: difference in AICW/trial; PPI analyses were used to investigate how the speech condition modulated functional connectivity between (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN. However, circularity was introduced because the networks were defined based on the control group. | Other: In controls, the L FTP network reduced connectivity with the DMN during speech, while the R FTP network increased connectivity with the DMN during speech. Both of these interactions were significantly decreased in patients. This was also true for contrasts 2 and 3. |
Griffis et al. (2016): Vox 1 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 Somewhat valid (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
UNR | UNR | Vox NC |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: none | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R thalamus ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R cerebellum notes: based on description in text; it is noted that no regions survived FDR correction |
Griffis et al. (2016): ROI 1 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 Somewhat valid (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix FDR |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) frontal LI; how ROIs defined: first principal component of 8 mm spheres defined based on previously reported control peaks; lesion volume included in model | ↑ L IFG ↓ R IFG ↑ LI (frontal) |
Griffis et al. (2016): ROI 2 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ semantic fluency Somewhat valid (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix FDR |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) frontal LI; how ROIs defined: first principal component of 8 mm spheres defined based on previously reported control peaks; lesion volume included in model | ↓ R IFG notes: decreased R IFG activation was correlated with improved semantic fluency |
Griffis et al. (2016): Cplx 1 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 Somewhat valid (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
PPI analyses were used to investigate change over time in modulation by verb generation of functional connectivity between L IFG and R IFG. | Other: There was a significant decrease in modulation by verb generation of functional connectivity between L IFG and R IFG (p = 0.03). Prior to TMS, connectivity increased during verb generation compared to finger tapping, while after TMS, connectivity decreased during verb generation compared to finger tapping. |
Griffis et al. (2016): Cplx 2 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ semantic fluency in association with modulation of interhemispheric IFG connectivity by verb generation Somewhat valid (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
PPI analyses were used to investigate whether change over time in modulation by verb generation of functional connectivity between L IFG and R IFG was associated with changes in semantic fluency scores, which are limited as a measure of language improvement. | None |
Griffis et al. (2016): Cplx 3 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 Somewhat valid (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
PPI analyses were used to investigate change over time in modulation by verb generation of functional connectivity between R IFG and all other brain regions. Voxelwise p < .001, not corrected for multiple comparisons. | Other: Reduced connectivity was observed in the L IFG pars opercularis, L anterior temporal lobe, L occipital lobe, L basal ganglia, R SMA and pre-SMA, R somato-motor cortex, R posterior MTG, and R cerebellum. It is noted that no regions survived FDR correction. |
Sims et al. (2016): ROI 1 |
Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: semantic feature decision accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 16; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars opercularis; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4) L SFG; (5) L MFG; (6) L MTG; (7) L AG/SMG; (8) L ACC; (9-16) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: AAL | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L IFG pars triangularis |
Sims et al. (2016): ROI 2 |
Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: WAB AQ |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 16; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars opercularis; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4) L SFG; (5) L MFG; (6) L MTG; (7) L AG/SMG; (8) L ACC; (9-16) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: AAL | None |
Sims et al. (2016): ROI 3 |
Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: BNT |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 16; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars opercularis; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4) L SFG; (5) L MFG; (6) L MTG; (7) L AG/SMG; (8) L ACC; (9-16) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: AAL | None |
Sims et al. (2016): ROI 4 |
Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: PPT |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 16; ROIs: (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars opercularis; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4) L SFG; (5) L MFG; (6) L MTG; (7) L AG/SMG; (8) L ACC; (9-16) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: AAL | None |
Sims et al. (2016): ROI 5 |
Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
Y | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Behavioral data notes: no correlation between lesion volume and accuracy, not clear whether control condition accuracy was also tested; number of ROIs: 8; ROIs: as above but only in the R hemisphere; how ROIs defined: AAL | ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior MTG notes: MTG included anterior too; SMG/AG was single ROI |
Sims et al. (2016): Cplx 1 |
Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion status of 8 ROIs |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Multivariate mixed-effects linear regression analyses were used to identify relationships between structural damage to 8 regions, and functional activation in 16 regions. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons based on FDR. This analysis was not described in sufficient detail. | Other: Sparing of the L ACC and L SFG was associated with more functional activation in many regions, however this is difficult to interpret since these regions were largely or completely spared in many patients. Damage to the L IFG pars orbitalis, L MTG and L AG/SMG was associated with activation of the L ACC, L SFG (and other regions) potentially indicative of compensatory processing. |
Sims et al. (2016): Cplx 2 |
Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Correlations were computed between functional activation in 16 regions, and qualitatively compared between patients and controls (p. 123). There was no correction for multiple comparisons. | Other: In controls, all regions were generally correlated with one another. This was largely true in patients too, with the exception of the R IFG pars orbitalis, which was negatively correlated with the L IFG. |
Darkow et al. (2017): Vox 1 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAA Aphasia after tDCS (n = 16) vs aphasia after sham stimulation (n = 16); same patients, order counterbalanced, repeated measures Somewhat valid (no behavioral difference) |
Y | Y | Vox C+ |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT; repeated measures | ↓ L insula ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ R occipital ↓ R anterior cingulate |
Darkow et al. (2017): ROI 1 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAC Aphasia after sham stimulation (n = 16) vs control |
AS | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients named > 90% correctly in all sessions; control RT not reported; number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) bilateral anterior cingulate; (2) L insula; (3) R lingual gyrus; how ROIs defined: regions that were less active in patients with tDCS vs sham; circular because ROIs defined in one group | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R anterior cingulate |
Darkow et al. (2017): ROI 2 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAC Aphasia after tDCS (n = 16) vs control |
AS | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients named > 90% correctly in all sessions; control RT not reported; number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) bilateral anterior cingulate; (2) L insula; (3) R lingual gyrus; how ROIs defined: regions that were less active in patients with tDCS vs sham; circular because ROIs defined in one group | None |
Darkow et al. (2017): Cplx 1 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAA Aphasia after tDCS (n = 16) vs aphasia after sham stimulation (n = 16); same patients, order counterbalanced, repeated measures Somewhat valid (no behavioral difference) |
Y | Y | Cplx |
ICA was used to derive three task-relevant components: language, motor and visual. Thresholding of the functional maps is not described, but they appear to reflect coherent components of a picture naming network. These components were compared between stimulation conditions in terms of mean activity and power in three frequency bins. It should be noted that the language component is left-lateralized, unlike the model-based picture naming contrast. | Other: Activity in the language component was greater in the tDCS condition. In the frequency domain, the tDCS condition showed reduced power in the highest frequency bin, and increased power in the lowest frequency bin. |
Darkow et al. (2017): Cplx 2 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAC Aphasia after sham stimulation (n = 16) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
ICA was used to derive three task-relevant components: language, motor and visual. Thresholding of the functional maps is not described, but they appear to reflect coherent components of a picture naming network. These components were compared between stimulation conditions in terms of mean activity and power in three frequency bins. It should be noted that the language component is left-lateralized, unlike the model-based picture naming contrast. | Other: Mean activity of these components did not differ between patients and controls. However, patients showed increased power in the middle frequency bin of the visual component. |
Darkow et al. (2017): Cplx 3 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAC Aphasia after tDCS (n = 16) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
ICA was used to derive three task-relevant components: language, motor and visual. Thresholding of the functional maps is not described, but they appear to reflect coherent components of a picture naming network. These components were compared between stimulation conditions in terms of mean activity and power in three frequency bins. It should be noted that the language component is left-lateralized, unlike the model-based picture naming contrast. | None |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): Vox 1 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia mean of T1, T2 Covariate: simultaneous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words Somewhat valid (potentially confounded by T1 and T2 language function; language function at T1 was predictive of change in language function) |
AM | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: FSL; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 1.6 cc | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior STS ↑ R anterior cingulate notes: findings based on figures and coordinates; the pre-SMA/dACC peak noted to survive FWE correction at p < .001 |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): ROI 1 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
N | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: number of AICW increased; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L pre-SMA; how ROI defined: peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia; no main effect of session in session by language recovery ANOVA | None |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): ROI 2 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ number of appropriate information-carrying words |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L pre-SMA; how ROI defined: peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia; no interaction of session by language recovery in ANOVA | None |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): ROI 3 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia mean of T1, T2 Covariate: simultaneous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words Somewhat valid (potentially confounded by T1 and T2 language function; language function at T1 was predictive of change in language function) |
AM | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L pre-SMA; how ROI defined: peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal notes: patients with more pre-SMA activity improved more |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): ROI 4 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia mean of T1, T2 Covariate: simultaneous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words Somewhat valid (potentially confounded by T1 and T2 language function; language function at T1 was predictive of change in language function) |
AM | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L pre-SMA; how ROI defined: peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia; lesion size covariate | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal notes: patients with more pre-SMA activity improved more |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): ROI 5 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia mean of T1, T2 Covariate: simultaneous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words |
AM | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L pre-SMA; how ROI defined: peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia; lesion size, T1 performance, and age covariates | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal notes: patients with more pre-SMA activity improved more |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): ROI 6 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia mean of T1, T2 Covariate: subsequent outcome (T2) number of appropriate information-carrying words Not valid (mathematically equivalent to the previous analysis, because of the inclusion of T1 performance as a covariate) |
AM | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L pre-SMA; how ROI defined: peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia; lesion size, T1 performance, and age covariates | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): ROI 7 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words Somewhat valid (potentially confounded by T1 language function; language function at T1 was predictive of change in language function) |
N | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L pre-SMA; how ROI defined: peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Geranmayeh et al. (2017): ROI 8 |
Propositional speech production vs rest | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: previous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words Not valid (the logic behind correlating activation changes and language outcome is unclear) |
AM | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L pre-SMA; how ROI defined: peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, & Szaflarski (2017): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: semantic decision accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Oth FWE |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) L AG and bilateral midline components of the canonical semantic network, along with reduced activity in R frontal, temporal and parietal regions; (2) bilateral IFG pars orbitalis; (3) L IFG and DLPFC along with bilateral midline regions; how ROIs defined: ROIs are mixing coefficients of functional networks arising from mCCA + jICA that were differently represented in the patient and control groups | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R posterior cingulate ↓ L insula ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsal precentral ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal notes: all 3 networks were significantly correlated; analysis of networks so involvement of each individual region cannot be assured |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, & Szaflarski (2017): ROI 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: average of semantic and phonemic fluency |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth FWE |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) L AG and bilateral midline components of the canonical semantic network, along with reduced activity in R frontal, temporal and parietal regions; (2) bilateral IFG pars orbitalis; (3) L IFG and DLPFC along with bilateral midline regions; how ROIs defined: ROIs are mixing coefficients of functional networks arising from mCCA + jICA that were differently represented in the patient and control groups | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R posterior cingulate ↓ L insula ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsal precentral ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal notes: networks 1 and 3 were significantly correlated; analysis of networks so involvement of each individual region cannot be assured |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, & Szaflarski (2017): ROI 3 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: BNT |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth FWE |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) L AG and bilateral midline components of the canonical semantic network, along with reduced activity in R frontal, temporal and parietal regions; (2) bilateral IFG pars orbitalis; (3) L IFG and DLPFC along with bilateral midline regions; how ROIs defined: ROIs are mixing coefficients of functional networks arising from mCCA + jICA that were differently represented in the patient and control groups | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R posterior cingulate ↓ L insula ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsal precentral ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal notes: networks 1 and 3 were significantly correlated; analysis of networks so involvement of each individual region cannot be assured |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, & Szaflarski (2017): Cplx 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported; Multimodal canonical correlation analysis (mCCA) and joint ICA were used to identify 3 joint ICs (structural/functional) that were differently represented in the patient and control groups. Although there was no correction for multiple comparisons when the functional maps were thresholded, the maps for the three networks each appeared to relate to coherent parts of the semantic network. | Other: The first joint IC comprised preservation of tissue in L posterior temporo-parietal region, activity in the L AG and bilateral midline components of the canonical semantic network, and reduced activity in R frontal, temporal and parietal regions. The second joint IC comprised preservation of tissue in the the L basal ganglia/insula region, and activity predominantly in the IFG pars orbitalis bilaterally. The third joint IC comprised preservation of the L IFG and activity in the L IFG and DLPFC along with bilateral midline regions. The first joint IC was considered to provide more robust evidence for structure-function relationships than the other two, because it was the only one where individual structural and functional mixing coefficients remained correlated even when lesion volume was included as a covariate. |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Vox 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: semantic decision accuracy |
C | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/in-house; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 126 voxels (size not stated); lesion volume covariate | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ L brainstem ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital ↑ R brainstem ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ L somato-motor notes: based on figure and table; larger activations are compelling; smaller activations are not due to lenient correction approach |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Vox 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: average of semantic and phonemic fluency |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/in-house; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 126 voxels (size not stated); lesion volume covariate | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STS ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L brainstem ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ R posterior STS notes: based on figure and table; larger activations are compelling; smaller activations are not due to lenient correction approach |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Vox 3 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: BNT |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/in-house; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 126 voxels (size not stated); lesion volume covariate | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum notes: based on figure and table; larger activations are compelling; smaller activations are not due to lenient correction approach |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Vox 4 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: R hemisphere; software: SPM12/in-house; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 126 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R orbitofrontal ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R cerebellum ↓ R thalamus notes: based on figure and table; larger activations are compelling; smaller activations are not due to lenient correction approach |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Behavioral data notes: semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported; number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) overall canonical semantic network (CSN); (2) L CSN; (3) R CSN; (4) mirror L CSN in R; (5) out-of-network CSN in R; how ROIs defined: control data; circular because ROI defined in one group | ↓ L IFG ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L angular gyrus ↓ L precuneus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L occipital ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R IFG ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R angular gyrus ↓ R precuneus ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R occipital ↓ R posterior cingulate ↓ R cerebellum notes: results are for whole networks of regions, so individual regions cannot be assured; out-of-network R regions not listed since they were not significant in ROI 5 (only in ROI 4) |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): ROI 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) overall canonical semantic network (CSN); (2) L CSN; (3) R CSN; (4) mirror L CSN in R; (5) out-of-network CSN in R; how ROIs defined: control data | None |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): ROI 3 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: semantic decision accuracy |
C | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: CSN; how ROI defined: control data; lesion volume covariate | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum notes: correlation calculated for the whole network of regions, so correlation of individual regions cannot be assured |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): ROI 4 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: average of semantic and phonemic fluency |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: CSN; how ROI defined: control data; lesion volume covariate | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum notes: correlation calculated for the whole network of regions, so correlation of individual regions cannot be assured |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): ROI 5 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: BNT |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: CSN; how ROI defined: control data; lesion volume covariate | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum notes: correlation calculated for the whole network of regions, so correlation of individual regions cannot be assured |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Cplx 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported; Correlations between activation magnitudes in the L and R canonical semantic network (CSN) were compared between groups. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. | None |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Cplx 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported; Correlations between activation magnitudes in the L CSN and R mirrored CSN were compared between groups. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. | Other: Correlations between activations in the L CSN and the mirrored L CSN in the R hemisphere were stronger in patients than controls. |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Cplx 3 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported; Correlations between activation magnitudes in the L CSN and R out-of-network homotopic regions were compared between groups. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. | Other: Correlations between activations in the L CSN and R out-of-network homotopic regions were stronger in patients than controls. |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Cplx 4 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported; The difference in activation between the L CSN and R CSN was compared between patients and controls. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. | None |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Cplx 5 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported; The difference in activation between the L CSN and mirror L CSN in the R was compared between patients and controls. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. | Other: The difference was smaller in patients. |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Cplx 6 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia vs control |
N | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported; The difference in activation between the R CSN and out-of-network homotopic regions in the R was compared between patients and controls. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. | Other: The difference was smaller in patients. |
Griffis, Nenert, Allendorfer, Vannest, et al. (2017): Cplx 7 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia Covariate: interactions of semantic fluency and naming measures by lesion size |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
For the 4 R hemisphere regions that were more activated in patients with larger lesions (SPM analysis 4), analyses were carried out to determine whether the semantic fluency or naming measures were differentially impacted by activation depending on whether lesions were larger or smaller. | Other: For 1 of the 4 regions (R SMA), there were significant interactions such that in patients with larger lesions, more activation was associated with higher semantic fluency scores and higher BNT scores, while in patients with smaller lesions, more activation was associated with lower fluency and BNT scores. There was a similar relationship with semantic fluency in the R IFG pars opercularis but only at p(FDR) = 0.07. |
Harvey et al. (2017): Vox 1 |
Picture naming vs viewing patterns | LA Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox NDC |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM8; qualitative comparison on pp. 138-9 | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R occipital ↓ R hippocampus/MTL notes: based on Figure 5 and Table 4 |
Nardo et al. (2017): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (all conditions, correct trials) vs rest | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
YCT | N | Vox VFWE |
Behavioral data notes: RT faster at T2; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nardo et al. (2017): ROI 1 |
Picture naming (untrained items, no cue, correct trials) vs picture naming (trained items, no cue, correct trials) | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: "a change in un-cued naming RT" (exact measure unclear) Somewhat valid (unclear whether behavioral measure is longitudinal) |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) R anterior insula; (2) R IFG; (3) dorsal anterior cingulate; (4) L premotor cortex; how ROIs defined: peaks (only with SVC) for the main effect of untrained (4 conditions) vs trained (4 conditions) in T2 aphasia; unclear what the behavioral measure was exactly | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R insula |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAA Aphasia CIAT T2 (n = 11) vs untreated T2 (n = 8) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
AS | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L brainstem ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R superior parietal notes: based on coordinates in Table 4 |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAA Aphasia CIAT T3 (n = 11) vs untreated T3 (n = 8) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
UNT | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R orbitofrontal ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R posterior STS notes: based on coordinates in Table 4 |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 3 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAA Aphasia CIAT T2 (n = 11) vs untreated T2 (n = 8) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↓ L precuneus ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R posterior STS ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus notes: based on coordinates in Table 4 |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 4 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAA Aphasia CIAT T3 (n = 11) vs untreated T3 (n = 8) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R basal ganglia ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ R posterior STS ↓ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 5 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia CIAT T1 (n = 11) vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 6 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia CIAT T2 (n = 11) vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 7 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia CIAT T3 (n = 11) vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R superior parietal ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R cerebellum |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 8 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia untreated T1 (n = 8) vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L angular gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R angular gyrus ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 9 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia untreated T2 (n = 8) vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R mid temporal ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L orbitofrontal ↓ L intraparietal sulcus ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ L brainstem ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R inferior parietal lobule ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 10 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia untreated T3 (n = 8) vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox CA |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but not significantly for the semantic decision task, and more so on the tone decision task; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R superior parietal ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L orbitofrontal ↓ L mid temporal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L cerebellum ↓ L hippocampus/MTL ↓ R angular gyrus ↓ R anterior temporal |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 11 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia CIAT T1 (n = 11) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L superior parietal ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 12 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia CIAT T2 (n = 11) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L anterior cingulate ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ L occipital ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 13 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia CIAT T3 (n = 11) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ L posterior cingulate ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R mid temporal |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 14 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia untreated T1 (n = 8) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L occipital ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R superior parietal |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 15 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia untreated T2 (n = 8) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ L occipital ↓ R superior parietal ↓ R occipital ↓ R cerebellum |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 16 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia untreated T3 (n = 8) vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L occipital ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R occipital ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L occipital ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R cerebellum ↓ R basal ganglia |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 17 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ BNT |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R brainstem ↑ R basal ganglia |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 18 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T3 vs T2 Covariate: Δ BNT Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior MTG ↑ R thalamus |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 19 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ BNT |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R mid temporal |
Nenert et al. (2017): Vox 20 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T3 vs T2 Covariate: Δ BNT Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 50 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R orbitofrontal |
Nenert et al. (2017): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LA Aphasia ANOVA including T1, T2, T3 |
AS | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) cerebellar LI; (4) fronto-parietal LI; (5) Broca's LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2017): ROI 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LAA (Aphasia CIAT (n = 11) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) vs (untreated (n = 8) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
AS | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) cerebellar LI; (4) fronto-parietal LI; (5) Broca's LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2017): ROI 3 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LA Aphasia ANOVA including T1, T2, T3 |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) cerebellar LI; (4) fronto-parietal LI; (5) Broca's LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2017): ROI 4 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LAA (Aphasia CIAT (n = 11) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) vs (untreated (n = 8) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) Somewhat valid (no treatment effect) |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) cerebellar LI; (4) fronto-parietal LI; (5) Broca's LI | None |
Qiu et al. (2017): Vox 1 |
Picture naming vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox CA |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 10 voxels (size not stated); in the footnote to Table 2, there is a reference to FWE correction with Monte Carlo simulation, but this is not described in the text, and the values in the table appear to be inconsistent with that | ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L occipital ↑ L thalamus ↑ R inferior parietal lobule ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↓ L IFG ↓ L orbitofrontal ↓ L somato-motor ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction notes: findings are based on coordinates, which in many cases do not match the labels assigned in the paper |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (silently name, correct trials) vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; search volume: whole brain gray matter; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT; threshold of z > 3.1 mentioned in results, but presume 2.3 based on methods and figure | ↑ R precuneus ↓ L occipital |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain gray matter; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT; threshold of z > 3.1 mentioned in results, but presume 2.3 based on methods and figure | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L precuneus ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R posterior STS ↓ L IFG ↓ L insula ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L intraparietal sulcus ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L hippocampus/MTL ↓ R intraparietal sulcus notes: labels based largely on text with some adjustments based on figures; overall pattern of decreased L activity and increased R activity is quite convincing |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): Vox 3 |
Picture naming (silently name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; search volume: whole brain gray matter; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L anterior temporal ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L supramarginal gyrus ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R somato-motor notes: L anterior temporal correlation remained significant after accounting for lesion load and other factors |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): Vox 4 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain gray matter; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L posterior STG ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior STS ↑ R occipital ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L angular gyrus notes: L IFG pars orbitalis, R pSTS, and R somato-motor correlations remained remained significant after accounting for lesion load and other factors; note that the pars orbitalis region is described as frontal pole in the paper but the coordinates and image support pars orbitalis |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): Vox 5 |
Picture naming (both phases, correct trials) vs picture naming (both phases, incorrect trials) | CB Aphasia with naming < 80% (n = 24) |
NBD | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain gray matter; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: ~.01 (z > 2.3); cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): ROI 1 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func FWE |
Number of ROIs: 11; ROIs: (1) right IPS; (2) left IPS; (3) left PTr; (4) left dPOp; (5) right superior motor cortex; (6) right ventral motor cortex; (7) right supramarginal sulcus; (8) left medial SMA; (9) right marginal sulcus; (10) left dorsal motor cortex; (11) right STS; how ROIs defined: regions that were activated for control > aphasia (ROIs 1-4) or aphasia > control (ROIs 5-11) | ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R posterior STS ↓ L IFG pars opercularis notes: the L IFG pars opercularis and the R posterior STS also contributed to predicting PNT scores even when lesion load on critical areas for picture naming, and several other variables, were included in multiple regression models |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): ROI 2 |
Picture naming (silently name, correct trials) vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L anterior temporal; how ROI defined: activity for covert naming correlated with naming ability in patients, after controlling for lesion and demographic factors | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): ROI 3 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) L frontal pole; (2) R postcentral gyrus; (3) R STS; how ROIs defined: activity for overt naming correlated with naming ability in patients, after controlling for lesion and demographic factors | ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior STS |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): Cplx 1 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion patterns identified with SVR-LSM |
YCT | UNR | Cplx |
SVR-LSM was used to identify regions of damage associated with activation of R pSTS ROI (defined based on SPM analysis 2). The results were thresholded at voxelwise p < .01 (CDT), cluster extent > 500 voxels. | Other: Damage to the L IFG pars opercularis was associated with more activity in the R pSTS. Damage to the L pSTS was associated with less activity in the R pSTS. |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017a): Cplx 2 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 26) Covariate: lesion patterns identified with SVR-LSM |
YCT | UNR | Cplx |
SVR-LSM was used to identify regions of damage associated with activation of L IFG pars opercularis ROI (defined based on SPM analysis 2). The results were thresholded at voxelwise p < .01 (CDT), cluster extent > 500 voxels. | Other: Damage to the L pSTG, L pSTS, and white matter underlying the L precuneus was associated with more activity in the L IFG pars opercularis. There were no regions associated with less activity. |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 1 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L cerebellum ↑ L thalamus ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R insula ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R basal ganglia ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L orbitofrontal ↓ L intraparietal sulcus ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex notes: based on Table 2 |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 2 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CAC Aphasia vs control |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R posterior MTG ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L mid temporal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L cerebellum ↓ L thalamus ↓ L hippocampus/MTL notes: based on Table 3 |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 3 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L occipital ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R occipital ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R brainstem ↑ R basal ganglia notes: based on Table 4, except for R frontal activations which are missing from the table, and were added based on the figure |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 4 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia Covariate: lesion volume |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L precuneus ↑ L occipital ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R mid temporal ↑ R occipital ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R basal ganglia ↑ R hippocampus/MTL notes: based on Table 4, except for bilateral occipital activations which are missing from the table, and were added based on the figure |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 5 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with IPS damage (n not stated) vs without IPS damage (n not stated) |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 6 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with insula damage (n = 18) vs without insula damage (n = 21) |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT; lesion volume covariate | ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 7 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 16) vs without IFG POp damage (n = 23) |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT; lesion volume covariate | ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 8 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with motor cortex damage (n = 24) vs without motor cortex damage (n = 15) |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): Vox 9 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with STS damage (n not stated) vs without STS damage (n not stated) |
YCT | UNR | Vox C- |
Search volume: whole brain; software: FSL 5.0.6; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 1 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 16) Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R DLPFC; how ROI defined: peak location for decreased activation for patients with left insula and left POp lesions compared to patients without said damage; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 2 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 23) Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R DLPFC; how ROI defined: peak location for decreased activation for patients with left insula and left POp lesions compared to patients without said damage; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 3 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia with insula damage (n = 18) Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R DLPFC; how ROI defined: peak location for decreased activation for patients with left insula and left POp lesions compared to patients without said damage; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 4 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia without insula damage (n = 21) Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func One |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R DLPFC; how ROI defined: peak location for decreased activation for patients with left insula and left POp lesions compared to patients without said damage; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 5 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with IPS damage (n not stated) vs without IPS damage (n not stated) |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) L IPS; (2) L insula; (3) L IFG pars opercularis; (4) R IPS; (5) R insula; how ROIs defined: 5 mm spheres around control peaks; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 6 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with insula damage (n = 18) vs without insula damage (n = 21) |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) L IPS; (2) L insula; (3) L IFG pars opercularis; (4) R IPS; (5) R insula; how ROIs defined: 5 mm spheres around control peaks; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 7 |
Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 16) vs without IFG POp damage (n = 23) |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase; number of ROIs: 5; ROIs: (1) L IPS; (2) L insula; (3) L IFG pars opercularis; (4) R IPS; (5) R insula; how ROIs defined: 5 mm spheres around control peaks; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 8 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with motor cortex damage (n = 24) vs without motor cortex damage (n = 15) |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L motor; (2) L pSTS; (3) R motor; (4) R pSTS; how ROIs defined: 5 mm spheres around control peaks; lesion volume covariate | ↑ R somato-motor |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 9 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with STS damage (n not stated) vs without STS damage (n not stated) |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L motor; (2) L pSTS; (3) R motor; (4) R pSTS; how ROIs defined: 5 mm spheres around control peaks; lesion volume covariate | ↓ R somato-motor |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 10 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia without motor cortex damage (n = 15) Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R motor; how ROI defined: 5 mm sphere around control peak; lesion volume covariate | None |
Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b): ROI 11 |
Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest | CC Aphasia with motor cortex damage (n = 24) Covariate: PNT |
YCT | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: R motor; how ROI defined: 5 mm sphere around control peak; lesion volume covariate | ↑ R somato-motor |
Dietz et al. (2018): ROI 1 |
Verb generation (overt) vs noun repetition | CAA Aphasia with AAC treatment (n = 6) T2 vs usual care T2 (n = 6) Somewhat valid (marginal treatment effect) |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: frontal LI; temporal LI calculated but not reported | None |
Dietz et al. (2018): ROI 2 |
Verb generation (overt) vs noun repetition | LC Aphasia (both groups) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ WAB AQ Somewhat valid (gain in AQ not tested for significance) |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: frontal LI; temporal LI calculated but not reported | ↑ LI (frontal) |
Hallam et al. (2018): ROI 1 |
Listening to high or low ambiguity sentences vs listening to spectrally rotated speech | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) L vATL; (2) L pMTG; how ROIs defined: functional coordinates in literature; ANOVA revealed main effect of group (patient vs control), confirmed in follow-up tests for each ROI | ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ L anterior temporal |
Hallam et al. (2018): ROI 2 |
Listening to high ambiguity sentences vs listening to low ambiguity sentences | CAC Aphasia vs control |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) L vATL; (2) L pMTG; how ROIs defined: functional coordinates in literature; no interaction of group by condition | None |
Hallam et al. (2018): Cplx 1 |
Listening to high ambiguity sentences vs listening to low ambiguity sentences | CAC Aphasia (subset with resting state data, n = 10) vs control (subset with resting state data, n = 10) |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
A whole brain analysis was carried out to identify regions where the groups differed in the extent to which the strength of functional connectivity at rest from L pMTG was associated with the difference in signal between the high ambiguity and low ambiguity conditions in the same ROI. Thresholding is not described and cluster extent is not reported. | Other: There was a functional activation by group interaction in the L aSTG. For controls, there was a positive association between L pMTG activity and functional connectivity to aSTG, while for the patients, there was a negative association. |
Hallam et al. (2018): Cplx 2 |
Listening to high ambiguity sentences vs listening to low ambiguity sentences | CAC Aphasia (subset with resting state data, n = 10) vs control (subset with resting state data, n = 10) |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
A whole brain analysis was carried out to identify regions where the groups differed in the extent to which the strength of functional connectivity at rest from L pMTG was associated with the difference in signal between the high ambiguity and low ambiguity conditions in the same ROI. Thresholding is not described. | None notes: no interaction is reported; both groups showed a correlation between L vATL activity and functional connectivity to a ventral IFG region |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox VP |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | ↑ L Heschl's gyrus |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T2 vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox VP |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 3 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T3 vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox VP |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 4 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T4 vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox VP |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 5 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T5 vs control |
AM | UNR | Vox VP |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 6 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 7 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T2 vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 8 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T3 vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 9 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T4 vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 10 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T5 vs control |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 11 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: semantic decision accuracy |
C | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | ↑ L anterior temporal notes: unclear why this type of analysis was run only for semantic task, and only at T1 |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 12 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ BNT |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 13 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ semantic fluency |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 14 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ PPVT |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 15 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ phonemic fluency |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 16 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ BDAE complex ideation subtest |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 17 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ BNT |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 18 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ semantic fluency |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R anterior temporal |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 19 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ PPVT |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 20 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ phonemic fluency |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | ↑ L cerebellum |
Nenert et al. (2018): Vox 21 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LC Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ BDAE complex ideation subtest |
UNR | UNR | Vox VP |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12/SnPM13; voxelwise p: FWE p < .05 | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LA Aphasia (comparisons between all pairs of time points) |
AS | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 2 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LA Aphasia (comparisons between all pairs of time points) |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 3 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
AM | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 4 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T2 vs control |
AM | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 5 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T3 vs control |
AM | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 6 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T4 vs control |
AM | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 7 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | CAC Aphasia T5 vs control |
AM | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Behavioral data notes: patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task; number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 8 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T1 vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 9 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T2 vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 10 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T3 vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 11 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T4 vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): ROI 12 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | CAC Aphasia T5 vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI LI NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Cplx 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LA Aphasia (comparisons between all pairs of time points) |
AS | UNR | Cplx |
PPI analyses were carried out to investigate potential changes over time in how connectivity from L and R IFG was modulated by the semantic decision task. The resultant SPM was thresholded at FWE p < .05 using permutation testing implemented in SnPM 13. | None |
Nenert et al. (2018): Cplx 2 |
Verb generation vs finger tapping | LA Aphasia (comparisons between all pairs of time points) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
PPI analyses were carried out to investigate potential changes over time in how connectivity from L and R IFG was modulated by the verb generation task. The resultant SPM was thresholded at FWE p < .05 using permutation testing implemented in SnPM 13. | None |
Pillay et al. (2018): Vox 1 |
Reading nouns aloud (correct trials) vs reading nouns aloud (incorrect trials) | CB Aphasia |
NBD | Y | Vox CCS |
Search volume: whole brain; software: AFNI; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 1.609 cc; regarding correction for multiple comparisons, addition of monoexponential function reduces but does not eliminate inflation of p values (Cox et al., 2017) | ↑ L angular gyrus ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R insula ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal notes: positive region (L AG) was part of the semantic network, while many negative regions were positively modulated by reaction time in the aphasia group |
Szaflarski et al. (2018): Vox 1 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 0.928 cc | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R superior parietal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R mid temporal ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L dorsal precentral ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L occipital |
Szaflarski et al. (2018): Vox 2 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LA Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 0.928 cc | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STS ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ R IFG ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R somato-motor ↓ R precuneus ↓ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ R anterior temporal |
Szaflarski et al. (2018): Vox 3 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LA Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 0.928 cc | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↓ L somato-motor ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Szaflarski et al. (2018): Vox 4 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T3 vs aphasia T2 Covariate: Δ WAB AQ |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 0.928 cc; inclusive mask of voxels that differed between T2 and T3 | ↓ L inferior parietal lobule |
Szaflarski et al. (2018): Vox 5 |
Semantic decision vs tone decision | LC Aphasia T3 vs aphasia T1 Covariate: Δ BNT |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCTB |
Search volume: whole brain; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .05; cluster extent cutoff: 0.928 cc; inclusive mask of voxels that differed between T1 and T3 | ↓ R IFG |
van de Sandt-Koenderman et al. (2018): ROI 1 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: lesion volume |
NANB | NANT | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: language network LI; how ROI defined: activations that were "not clearly related to known language areas" were excluded, but the basis for this determination is not clear | None |
van de Sandt-Koenderman et al. (2018): ROI 2 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: lesion volume |
NANB | NANT | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: language network LI; how ROI defined: activations that were "not clearly related to known language areas" were excluded, but the basis for this determination is not clear | None |
van de Sandt-Koenderman et al. (2018): ROI 3 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ AAT repetition score |
NANB | NANT | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: language network LI; how ROI defined: activations that were "not clearly related to known language areas" were excluded, but the basis for this determination is not clear | None |
van de Sandt-Koenderman et al. (2018): ROI 4 |
Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ ANELT |
NANB | NANT | ROI LI One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: language network LI; how ROI defined: activations that were "not clearly related to known language areas" were excluded, but the basis for this determination is not clear | None |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 1 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T1 (n = 10) Covariate: subsequent outcome (T4) overall language measure (average of AAT measures) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 12; ROIs: (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected | ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus notes: activation predicted later outcome even when initial language performance was included in the model |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 2 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia (all time points) Covariate: overall language measure (average of AAT measures) all time points |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 12; ROIs: (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected; mixed model; minimal detail provided | ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 3 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia (all time points) Covariate: average of AAT comprehension score and BNT, all time points |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 12; ROIs: (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected; mixed model; minimal detail provided | ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 4 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | CC Aphasia (all time points) Covariate: picture-word matching accuracy, all time points |
C | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 12; ROIs: (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected; mixed model; minimal detail provided | ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 5 |
Written word-picture matching vs visual decision | LA Aphasia: linear effect of time |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 12; ROIs: (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected; mixed model; minimal detail provided | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R anterior cingulate ↑ R thalamus ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis notes: similar numbers of findings are reported for controls |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 6 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T1 (n = 10) Covariate: subsequent outcome (T4) overall language measure (average of AAT measures) Somewhat valid (not appropriate to correlate T1 imaging with T4 behavior without T1 behavior in model) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected | None |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 7 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia (all time points) Covariate: overall language measure (average of AAT measures) all time points |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected; mixed model; minimal detail provided | None |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 8 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia (all time points) Covariate: average of AAT comprehension score and BNT, all time points |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected; mixed model; minimal detail provided | None |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 9 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | CC Aphasia (all time points) Covariate: semantic decision accuracy, all time points |
C | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected; mixed model; minimal detail provided | None |
van Oers et al. (2018): ROI 10 |
Semantic decision vs visual decision | LA Aphasia: linear effect of time |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func FDR |
Number of ROIs: 6; ROIs: (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG; how ROIs defined: control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected; mixed model; minimal detail provided | ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis notes: similar numbers of findings are reported for controls |
Barbieri et al. (2019): Vox 1 |
Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures | LA Aphasia treated (n = 13) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCS |
Behavioral data notes: out-of-scanner performance on passive sentences improved; software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 37 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L precuneus ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R superior parietal ↑ R precuneus notes: based on Table 7 and Figure 8 |
Barbieri et al. (2019): Vox 2 |
Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures | LA Aphasia natural history (n = 5) T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | Vox CCS |
Software: SPM8; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: 37 voxels (size not stated) | None |
Barbieri et al. (2019): ROI 1 |
Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures | LAA (Aphasia treated (n=13) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia natural history (n=5) T2 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L hemisphere sentence processing network (IFGpt, pMTG, pSTG, AG); (2) R hemisphere homotopic regions; (3) L dorsal attention network (MFG, PrCG, SPL, sLOC); (4) R dorsal attention network (same regions); how ROIs defined: sentence processing network based on Walenski et al. (2019); dorsal attention network based on Corbetta et al. (2008) and Vincent et al. (2008); ROIs were defined based on Harvard-Oxford atlas which would align imperfectly with these functional networks; dependent variable was number of active voxels (p < .001, uncorrected) divided by number of intact voxels; derivation of dependent measures from ROIs difficulty to follow, but it seems that ROIs with less than 5 voxels upregulated were excluded and deactivations were not considered, meaning that estimates of change may be biased | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R superior parietal notes: bilateral dorsal attention network; findings were for networks as a whole; regions coded correspond to atlas ROIs |
Barbieri et al. (2019): ROI 2 |
Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: ∆ offline comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L hemisphere sentence processing network (IFGpt, pMTG, pSTG, AG); (2) R hemisphere homotopic regions; (3) L dorsal attention network (MFG, PrCG, SPL, sLOC); (4) R dorsal attention network (same regions); how ROIs defined: sentence processing network based on Walenski et al. (2019); dorsal attention network based on Corbetta et al. (2008) and Vincent et al. (2008); ROIs were defined based on Harvard-Oxford atlas which would align imperfectly with these functional networks; dependent variable was number of active voxels (p < .001, uncorrected) divided by number of intact voxels; derivation of dependent measures from ROIs difficulty to follow, but it seems that ROIs with less than 5 voxels upregulated were excluded and deactivations were not considered, meaning that estimates of change may be biased | ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R superior parietal ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG notes: R homotopic sentence processing network and R dorsal attention network; findings were for networks as a whole; regions coded correspond to atlas ROIs |
Barbieri et al. (2019): ROI 3 |
Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures | LC Aphasia participants with eye tracking data (n = 16) T2 vs T1 Covariate: ∆ decrease in eye tracking online thematic prediction score |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L hemisphere sentence processing network (IFGpt, pMTG, pSTG, AG); (2) R hemisphere homotopic regions; (3) L dorsal attention network (MFG, PrCG, SPL, sLOC); (4) R dorsal attention network (same regions); how ROIs defined: sentence processing network based on Walenski et al. (2019); dorsal attention network based on Corbetta et al. (2008) and Vincent et al. (2008); ROIs were defined based on Harvard-Oxford atlas which would align imperfectly with these functional networks; dependent variable was number of active voxels (p < .001, uncorrected) divided by number of intact voxels; derivation of dependent measures from ROIs difficulty to follow, but it seems that ROIs with less than 5 voxels upregulated were excluded and deactivations were not considered, meaning that estimates of change may be biased | ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG notes: R homotopic sentence processing network; findings were for networks as a whole; regions coded correspond to atlas ROIs |
Barbieri et al. (2019): ROI 4 |
Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures | LC Aphasia participants with eye tracking data (n = 16) T2 vs T1 Covariate: ∆ eye tracking online thematic integragration score |
UNR | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L hemisphere sentence processing network (IFGpt, pMTG, pSTG, AG); (2) R hemisphere homotopic regions; (3) L dorsal attention network (MFG, PrCG, SPL, sLOC); (4) R dorsal attention network (same regions); how ROIs defined: sentence processing network based on Walenski et al. (2019); dorsal attention network based on Corbetta et al. (2008) and Vincent et al. (2008); ROIs were defined based on Harvard-Oxford atlas which would align imperfectly with these functional networks; dependent variable was number of active voxels (p < .001, uncorrected) divided by number of intact voxels; derivation of dependent measures from ROIs difficulty to follow, but it seems that ROIs with less than 5 voxels upregulated were excluded and deactivations were not considered, meaning that estimates of change may be biased | ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R superior parietal notes: R dorsal attention network; findings were for networks as a whole; regions coded correspond to atlas ROIs |
Johnson et al. (2019): ROI 1 |
Picture naming (trained items) vs rest | CAC Aphasia treated T1 (n = 26) vs control |
N | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 16; ROIs: (1) L IFGorb; (2) L IFGtri; (3) L IFGop; (4) L MFG; (5) L PrCG; (6) L MTG; (7) L SMG; (8) L AG; (9-16) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: AAL but lesioned voxels were excluded from ROIs on an individual basis | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ L angular gyrus notes: significant interaction of ROI by group |
Johnson et al. (2019): ROI 2 |
Picture naming (trained items) vs rest | CAC Aphasia treated T2 (n = 26) vs control |
N | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 16; ROIs: (1) L IFGorb; (2) L IFGtri; (3) L IFGop; (4) L MFG; (5) L PrCG; (6) L MTG; (7) L SMG; (8) L AG; (9-16) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: AAL but lesioned voxels were excluded from ROIs on an individual basis | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R IFG pars triangularis notes: significant interaction of ROI by group; patients also showed more activity than controls across the average of all ROIs |
Johnson et al. (2019): ROI 3 |
Picture naming (trained items) vs rest | LA Aphasia untreated (n = 10) T2 vs T1 |
Y | UNR | ROI Anat NC |
Number of ROIs: 16; ROIs: (1) L IFGorb; (2) L IFGtri; (3) L IFGop; (4) L MFG; (5) L PrCG; (6) L MTG; (7) L SMG; (8) L AG; (9-16) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: AAL but lesioned voxels were excluded from ROIs on an individual basis | None notes: no main effect of time or interaction of time by ROI |
Johnson et al. (2019): Cplx 1 |
Picture naming (trained items) vs rest | LA Aphasia treated (n = 26) T2 vs T1 |
N | UNR | Cplx |
A linear model was constructed to examine the relationship between proportion of spared tissue in each L hemisphere ROI and changes in activation over time. The model is not described in sufficient detail. | Other: There was a significant 3-way interaction of time by ROI by spared tissue, such that in some regions (AG, MFG, IFG orb, SMG), less spared tissue was associated with greater increases in activation, while in others (PrCG, IFG op, IFG tri), less spared tissue was associated with greater decreases in activation. |
Kristinsson et al. (2019): Vox 1 |
Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures | CAA Aphasia with typical genotype (n = 53) vs atypical genotype (n = 34) |
Y | UNR | Vox VFWE |
Software: SPM12 | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): Vox 1 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LA Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
AM | AM | Vox CCS |
Behavioral data notes: see section S2, but main effects include known items also; search volume: appears to be restricted to voxels spared in all patients; software: BrainVoyager QX 2.4 or SPM12; voxelwise p: .01; cluster extent cutoff: 49 voxels (size not stated) | ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior cingulate |
Purcell et al. (2019): ROI 1 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ spelling accuracy on training items |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) R AG; (2) L PCC; (3) R PCC; how ROIs defined: regions activated in SPM analysis 1 | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): ROI 2 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 3; ROIs: (1) R AG; (2) L PCC; (3) R PCC; how ROIs defined: regions activated in SPM analysis 1 | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): ROI 3 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ spelling accuracy on training items (T2 vs T1) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ventral occipitotemporal cortex; how ROI defined: the region that showed an increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): ROI 4 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | CC Aphasia with both timepoints T1 (n = 20) Covariate: subsequent Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items (T2 vs T1) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ventral occipitotemporal cortex; how ROI defined: the region that showed an increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): ROI 5 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ spelling accuracy on training items |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ventral occipitotemporal cortex; how ROI defined: the region that showed an increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): ROI 6 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func One |
Number of ROIs: 1; ROI: L ventral occipitotemporal cortex; how ROI defined: the region that showed an increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 1 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LA Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
AM | AM | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: see section S2, where Figures S1 and S2 appear to show differences; the main effects of time were not significant for accuracy or RT, but those analyses included known items also, which had smaller effects; Local Heterogeneity Regression Analysis (Local-Hreg) was used to identify brain regions where the heterogeneity of timecourses between neighboring voxels, specifically for the trained condition, increased from T1 to T2. A voxelwise threshold of p < 0.05 was applied, followed by cluster correction based on permutation testing. The analysis appears to have been restricted to brain regions not damaged in any patients. | Other: Only in L ventral occipitotemporal cortex, there was a significant increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 (p = 0.028, corrected). |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 2 |
Spelling probe (known items) vs rest | LA Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Y | Y | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: see section S2, main effects were not significant and effects appear smaller for known than trained; Local Heterogeneity Regression Analysis (Local-Hreg) was used to identify brain regions where the heterogeneity of timecourses between neighboring voxels, specifically for the known condition, increased from T1 to T2. A voxelwise threshold of p < 0.05 was applied, followed by cluster correction based on permutation testing. The analysis appears to have been restricted to brain regions not damaged in any patients. | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 3 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: T1 spelling accuracy on training items Somewhat valid (training items were selected for individual patients, so training item accuracy is not an appropriate measure of spelling ability) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between Local-Hreg at T1 in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and T1 spelling accuracy of training items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | Other: There was a significant positive relationship between T1 Local-Hreg and T1 spelling accuracy on training items. |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 4 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: subsequent Δ spelling accuracy on training items (T2 vs T1) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between Local-Hreg at T1 in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and subsequent improvement in spelling accuracy of training items from T1 to T2. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | Other: There was a significant positive relationship between T1 Local-Hreg and subsequent improvement in spelling accuracy on training items from T1 to T2. |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 5 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | CC Aphasia with both timepoints T1 (n = 20) Covariate: subsequent Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items (T2 vs T1) Somewhat valid (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between Local-Hreg at T1 in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and subsequent improvement in spelling accuracy of untrained items from T1 to T2. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | Other: There was a significant positive relationship between T1 Local-Hreg and subsequent improvement in spelling accuracy on untrained items from T1 to T2. |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 6 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ spelling accuracy on training items |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and change in spelling accuracy of training items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | Other: There was a significant negative relationship between change in Local-Hreg and change in spelling accuracy on training items. |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 7 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and change in spelling accuracy of untrained items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | Other: There was a significant negative relationship between change in Local-Hreg and change in spelling accuracy on untrained items. |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 8 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | CC Aphasia with both timepoints T2 (n = 20) Covariate: T2 spelling accuracy on training items |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between Local-Hreg at T2 in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and T2 spelling accuracy of training items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 9 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: previous T1 Local-Hreg in L ventral occipitotemporal ROI Not valid (the ROI was defined based on change in Local-Hreg, so spurious findings could arise in the absence of a real effect) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and T1 Local-Hreg. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | Other: There was a significant negative relationship between change in Local-Hreg and T1 Local-Hreg. |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 10 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ spelling accuracy on training items |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the R AG, L PCC, and R PCC and change in spelling accuracy of training items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | None |
Purcell et al. (2019): Cplx 11 |
Spelling probe (training items) vs rest | LC Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the R AG, L PCC, and R PCC and change in spelling accuracy of untrained items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. | None |
Sreedharan, Chandran, et al. (2019): ROI 1 |
Neurofeedback (try to activate language areas) vs rest | CAC Aphasia mean of T1, T2, T3, T4, T5, T6 (neurofeedback patients) or T1, T2 (no training patients) vs control mean |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func NDC |
Number of ROIs: 4; ROIs: (1) L Broca's area (IFG pars opercularis and triangularis); (2) L Wernicke's area (pSTG); (3-4) homotopic counterparts; how ROIs defined: individual activations within AAL ROIs on a separate word generation localizer | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L posterior STG ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R posterior STG |
Sreedharan, Chandran, et al. (2019): ROI 2 |
Neurofeedback (try to activate language areas) vs rest | CAA Aphasia with neurofeedback training (n = 4) mean of T4, T5, T6 vs no training (n = 4) T2 Somewhat valid (no treatment effect; second half measures rather than measures of change) |
NANB | NANT | ROI Func NC |
Number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L Broca's area (IFG pars opercularis and triangularis); (2) L Wernicke's area (pSTG); (3-4) homotopic counterparts; (5) L MFG; (6) L PrCG; (7) L Rolandic operculum; (8) L insula; (9) L IFG pars orbitalis; (10) L MFG orbital; (11) L SMG; (12) L MTG; (13) L PoCG; (14) L AG; (15) L HG; how ROIs defined: (1-4) individual activations within AAL ROIs on a separate word generation localizer; (5-15) AAL | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L somato-motor |
Sreedharan, Chandran, et al. (2019): Cplx 1 |
Neurofeedback (try to activate language areas) vs rest | CAC Aphasia mean of T1, T2, T3, T4, T5, T6 (neurofeedback patients) or T1, T2 (no training patients) vs control mean |
NANB | NANT | Cplx |
Signal change in L IFG and L pSTG ROIs was computed, along with functional connectivity between these ROIs. Neurofeedback values were calculated based on signal change as well as correlation between the ROIs. Group differences in neurofeedback values were compared, but not quantified statistically. | Other: Patients received lower neurofeedback values than controls, due to lower signal changes and lower functional connectivity. |
Hartwigsen et al. (2020): Vox 1 |
Syllable count decision vs rest | CAA Aphasia after cTBS to posterior IFG vs sham; same patients, repeated measures |
Y | N | Vox C+ |
Behavioral data notes: significantly slower response times when cTBS was applied over pIFG relative to when sham cTBS was applied; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R basal ganglia notes: based on Figure 4A and Table 3 |
Hartwigsen et al. (2020): Vox 2 |
Syllable count decision vs rest | CAA Aphasia after cTBS to posterior IFG vs after cTBS to anterior IFG; same patients, repeated measures |
Y | N | Vox C+ |
Behavioral data notes: significantly slower response times when cTBS was applied over pIFG relative to when cTBS was applied over aIFG; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis notes: based on Table 3 |
Hartwigsen et al. (2020): Vox 3 |
Semantic decision vs rest | CAA Aphasia after cTBS to anterior IFG vs sham; same patients, repeated measures Somewhat valid (no behavioral difference) |
Y | Y | Vox C+ |
Behavioral data notes: difference in reaction time did not survive correction; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↓ L insula ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal notes: based on Figure 4B and Table 3 |
Hartwigsen et al. (2020): Vox 4 |
Semantic decision vs rest | CAA Aphasia after cTBS to anterior IFG vs after cTBS to posterior IFG ; same patients, repeated measures |
Y | N | Vox C+ |
Behavioral data notes: significantly slower response times when cTBS was applied over aIFG relative to when cTBS was applied over pIFG; search volume: voxels spared in all patients; software: SPM12; voxelwise p: .001; cluster extent cutoff: based on GRFT | ↓ L insula ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex notes: based on Table 3 |
Hartwigsen et al. (2020): Cplx 1 |
Syllable count decision vs rest | CC Aphasia after cTBS to posterior IFG vs sham; same patients, repeated measures Covariate: Δ RT for syllable decision (cTBS to posterior IFG timepoint vs sham timepoint) |
UNR | C | Cplx |
Whole brain correlations were computed between the difference in functional activity after cTBS to posterior IFG versus sham stimulation, and the difference in reaction times on the syllable counting task under these two conditions. The resulting SPM was thresholded at voxelwise p < .001 (CDT) followed by correction for multiple comparisons based on cluster extent and GRFT using SPM12. | Other: Upregulation of the R supramarginal gyrus after cTBS was significantly associated with slowing of RT after cTBS. This finding remained significant after including lesion volume as covariate. |
Hartwigsen et al. (2020): Cplx 2 |
Semantic decision vs rest | CC Aphasia after cTBS to anterior IFG vs sham; same patients, repeated measures Covariate: Δ RT for semantic decision (cTBS to posterior IFG timepoint vs sham timepoint) |
UNR | C | Cplx |
Whole brain correlations were computed between the difference in functional activity after cTBS to anterior IFG versus sham stimulation, and the difference in reaction times on the semantic decision task under these two conditions. The resulting SPM was thresholded at voxelwise p < .001 (CDT) followed by correction for multiple comparisons based on cluster extent and GRFT using SPM12. | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 1 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; post-hoc tests comparing 2 out of the 3 time points were corrected using the Bonferroni-Holm procedure, but there is no indication that that multiple comparisons across ROIs were accounted for | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L insula ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R insula notes: based on Figure 3; several additional regions are mentioned in text and/or Table 1 |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 2 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; post-hoc tests comparing 2 out of the 3 time points were corrected using the Bonferroni-Holm procedure, but there is no indication that that multiple comparisons across ROIs were accounted for | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ L anterior temporal notes: based on Figure 3; several additional regions are mentioned in text and/or Table 1 |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 3 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; post-hoc tests comparing 2 out of the 3 time points were corrected using the Bonferroni-Holm procedure, but there is no indication that that multiple comparisons across ROIs were accounted for | None notes: based on Figure 3; several additional regions are mentioned in text and/or Table 1 |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 4 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal mean of T1, T2, T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal mean of T1, T2, T3 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints | ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R anterior temporal ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex notes: based on Table 1 |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 5 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; interactions were significant in model with all 3 time points; post-hoc sub-interactions not reported but the patterns appear clear | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 6 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; interactions were significant in model with all 3 time points; post-hoc sub-interactions not reported and patterns are not clear | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 7 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; post-hoc sub-interactions not reported but there do not appear to be any T2/T3 effects | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 8 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions; test of group by time interaction not reported | Other: there was a significant increase in activation in perilesional ROIs |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 9 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions; test of group by time interaction not reported | Other: there was a significant increase in activation in perilesional ROIs |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 10 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LA Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions; test of group by time interaction not reported | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 11 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal mean of T1, T2, T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal mean of T1, T2, T3 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions; test of group by time interaction not reported; this comparison is somewhat questionable given the differing extent to which frontal and temporal regions are activated in controls | Other: frontal patients showed relatively greater activation in regions homotopic to their lesions |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 12 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L insula ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 13 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L insula ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ R IFG pars triangularis |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 14 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints | ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R anterior temporal |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 15 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 16 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 17 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 18 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L insula |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 19 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 20 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | ROI Func NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 13; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; how ROIs defined: spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 21 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | Other: frontal patients showed reduced activation in perilesional tissue |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 22 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | Other: frontal patients showed reduced activation in perilesional tissue |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 23 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | Other: frontal patients showed reduced activation in perilesional tissue |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 24 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | Other: temporal patients showed reduced activation in perilesional tissue and in regions homotopic to their lesions |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 25 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 26 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAC Aphasia temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) vs control |
UNR | UNR | ROI Oth NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 2; ROIs: (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals; how ROIs defined: (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 27 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis other: L IFG pars opercularis and orbitalis did not remain significant when lesion volume was included as a covariate; there was a significant correlation between perilesional activation and LRScomp; this did not remain significant when lesion volume was included as a covariate |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 28 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis other: there was a significant correlation between perilesional activation and LRScomp |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 29 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T3 Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis notes: did not remain significant when lesion volume was included as a covariate |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 30 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | ↑ L insula ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex notes: R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex did not remain significant when lesion volume was included as a covariate |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 31 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T3 vs T1 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 32 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T3 vs T2 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 33 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 34 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 35 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 36 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 37 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T1 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 38 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T2 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 39 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | ↑ R anterior temporal |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 40 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 41 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) Covariate: comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 42 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia temporo-parietal (n = 17) T2 vs T1 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | ↑ L insula |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 43 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T1 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 44 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T2 Covariate: Δ comprehension composite |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 45 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis notes: lesion volume negatively correlated with activation |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 46 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T2 Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 47 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T3 Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 48 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 49 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T3 vs T1 Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): ROI 50 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T3 vs T2 Covariate: lesion volume |
UNR | UNR | ROI Mix NC |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; number of ROIs: 15; ROIs: (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions; how ROIs defined: (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 1 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | Other: Correlations were higher in the temporal group in the R ATL. |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 2 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | Other: Correlations were higher in the temporal group in L posterior temporal cortex and L IFG op. |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 3 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | Other: Correlations were different between groups in the R ATL, but the correlation is not reported as significant in the temporo-parietal group alone. |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 4 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia temporo-parietal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and changes in LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | Other: In the L insula, the temporo-parietal group showed a stronger correlation than the frontal group between changes in activation and changes in LRScomp. |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 5 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and changes in LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | Other: In the L insula, the temporo-parietal group showed a stronger correlation than the frontal group between changes in activation and changes in LRScomp. |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 6 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and changes in LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 7 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 8 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 9 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CAA Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 10 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 11 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 12 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LAA (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. | None |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 13 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | CC Aphasia T1 Covariate: interaction of comprehension composite by lesion size |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; To investigate why some activation-behavior relationships did not remain significant when lesion extent was included as a covariate, models were constructed looking at the relationship between activation and behavior in patients with larger and smaller lesions. | Other: The three regions where this applied at T1, namely perilesional cortex, L IFG op, and L IFG orb, all showed positive correlations between activation and LRScomp in patients with larger lesions, but no correlations in patients with smaller lesions. |
Stockert et al. (2020): Cplx 14 |
Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech | LC Aphasia T2 vs T1 Covariate: interaction of Δ comprehension composite by lesion size |
UNR | UNR | Cplx |
Behavioral data notes: no differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions; To investigate why some activation-behavior relationships did not remain significant when lesion extent was included as a covariate, models were constructed looking at the relationship between activation and behavior in patients with larger and smaller lesions. | Other: This applied to the R DLPFC in the T2 vs T1 analysis. This region showed a positive correlation between activation and LRScomp in patients with larger lesions, but no correlation in patients with smaller lesions. |