Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Lesion including L pSTG; moderate-to-severe Wernicke's aphasia in the subacute period; now recovered and not aphasic per formal testing; able to perform verb generation task |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 6 |
Number of control participants | 6 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (mean 58 years, range 50-66 years; controls were younger: mean 35 years; range 27-50 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 6; females: 0) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 6; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 5-117 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | Recovered; not aphasic per formal testing |
Aphasia type | Recovered, but all had moderate-severe Wernicke's aphasia in the subacute period |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | Posterior L MCA infarct, lesion to the L posterior STG usually extending to MTG and AG |
Participants notes | 6 patients were selected from a database of 600 carefully documented cases |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI ECAT 953/15) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 6 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (axial; field of view = 5.4 cm; perisylvian only) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L posterior temporal, IFG and ventral precentral gyrus, much smaller activations in the R hemisphere |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Pseudoword repetition |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | L posterior temporal only; similar but less extensive activation in the R hemisphere |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | In practice trials, patients produced 1.5 words on average per prompt, not all of which were verbs, while controls 2.3 words on average per prompt, almost all of which were verbs |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Perisylvian |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 729 (the word "significant" is used) |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Findings notes | Based more on Figure 2 than the text |
First level contrast | Pseudoword repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | All participants are reported to have had no difficulties in performing the repetition task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Perisylvian |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 729 (the word "significant" is used) |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R IFG ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Findings notes | Based more on Figure 2 than the text |
Language | French |
Inclusion criteria | MCA; persistent severe non-fluent aphasia followed by marked improvement with MIT |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 7 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 49.7 years, range 40-58 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 7; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 15-149 months; including MIT for the most recent 1-108 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | BDAE |
Aphasia severity | Persistent severe non-fluent aphasia followed by marked improvement with MIT |
Aphasia type | 5 global, 2 Broca's |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated, but note that hypoperfusion greatly exceeded the infarct in all but 1 patient |
Lesion location | L MCA; 2 also had ACA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CEA LETI-TTV03) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 4 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (7 transaxial slices 12 mm apart) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word repetition with MIT-like intonation |
Control condition | Word repetition |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | More words were correctly repeated with MIT (16.3 ± 8) than without (12.4 ± 8; p < 0.03) |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | N/A |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | N/A |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | N/A |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition with MIT-like intonation vs word repetition |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional performance-defined conditions |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | More words were correctly repeated with MIT (16.3 ± 8) than without (12.4 ± 8; p < 0.03) |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 18 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L Broca's area; (2) L prefrontal; (3) L sensorimotor mouth; (4) L parietal; (5) L Wernicke's area; (6) L Heschl's gyrus; (7) L anterior STG; (8) L MTG; (9) L temporal pole; (10-18) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images; activation quantified as mean rCBF, not including any intersection of the infarct with the ROI |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Three left hemisphere ROIs were excluded (3, 6, 9) because they were completely infarcted in 4 or more patients |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R posterior STG |
Findings notes | — |
Language | Japanese |
Inclusion criteria | Able to repeat single words |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 16 |
Number of control participants | 6 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 56.6 ± 11.8 years, range 38-75 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 12; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 16; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No* (moderate limitation) (mean 15.1 ± 16.7 months, range 1.1-50.3 months; a mix of subacute and chronic participants; 8 of each) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB |
Aphasia severity | AQ mean 74.3 ± 12.2, range 53.8-92.4 |
Aphasia type | 6 anomic, 4 atypical, 4 mild Broca's, 1 mild Wernicke's, 1 transcortical sensory; alternately: 10 fluent, 6 non-fluent |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Extent and location |
Lesion extent | Mean 33.9 ± 26.3 cc, range 8.1-113.2 cc |
Lesion location | L perisylvian |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Headtome IV tomograph) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 6 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | No (91 mm field of view; coverage limitations not stated) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word repetition |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral auditory and motor activations are prominent, only slightly L-lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Some of the patients made a few errors, so as a group they may have been less accurate than controls |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | The rCBF increase in R PIF was also significant at p < 0.005 for nonfluent patients with Fisher's protected least-significant difference |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia fluent (n = 10) vs non-fluent (n = 6) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ R IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Spontaneous speech (WAB) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | No correction for multiple comparisons across WAB subscores |
Findings | ↑ L IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Comprehension (WAB) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | This non-significant finding is implied but not stated explicitly |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Repetition (WAB) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | This non-significant finding is implied but not stated explicitly |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Naming (WAB) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L posterior inferior frontal; (2) R posterior inferior frontal; (3) L posterior superior temporal; (4) R posterior superior temporal; (5) L rolandic; (6) R rolandic; (7) SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | This non-significant finding is implied but not stated explicitly |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 6 |
Number of control participants | 6 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 33-66 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 4; females: 2) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 6; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (T1: ~4 weeks; T2: ~12-18 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | Verbal repetition, confrontation naming, oral and written comprehension, reading abilities, TT, phonemic fluency, clinical impression, family interview |
Aphasia severity | T1: TT range 37-48; T2: TT range 3-39 (1 missing) |
Aphasia type | T1: 5 global, 1 Wernicke's; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 27.2-133.2 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA; 5 patients had superior temporal damage and 1 had subcortical damage underlying posterior superior temporal cortex |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCMRgl) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: ~4 weeks; T2: ~12-18 months |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Not stated |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens ECAT EXACT HR) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 2 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | N/A—no intersubject normalization |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word repetition |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | The only control data is extent of activation and mean signal increase in L and R superior temporal cortex; both of these measures were slightly L-lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia with good recovery (n = 3) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with poor recovery (n = 3) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | not stated |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative generalization across individuals on pp. 214-6 |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ R posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Findings notes | The consistent aspects of the findings were that there was an emergence of L posterior temporal activation in patients with better recovery, and R posterior temporal activation in patients with worse recovery |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia with good recovery (n = 3) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with poor recovery (n = 3) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L superior temporal cortex; (2) R superior temporal cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images; activation quantified in terms of extent exceeding 10% signal change, and mean % increase over the activation |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative generalization across individuals on pp. 214, 216 |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | MCA; able to repeat single words |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 12 |
Number of control participants | 10 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (mean 57 years, range 34-78 years; controls not matched for age) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 7; females: 5; stated to be not matched, but difference not significant) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 12; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (T1: mean 24 ± 11 days, ~3-4 weeks; T2: mean 19 ± 2 months, > 1 year) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | TT |
Aphasia severity | T1: 9 severe; 2 mild; 1 not stated; TT range 3-47 errors; T2: not stated |
Aphasia type | T1: 8 global, 3 anomic, 1 Wernicke's; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Extent and location |
Lesion extent | Range 2-133 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | Only 7 of the 12 patients took part at T2 |
Modality | PET (rCMRgl) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: mean 24 ± 11 days, ~3-4 weeks; T2: mean 19 ± 2 months, > 1 year |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Not stated |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (activation and control images not acquired on the same day; number of acquisitions not clearly described) |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 8 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | N/A—no intersubject normalization |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word repetition |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | ROIs only; negligible evidence of lateralization |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 8 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) L STG/HG; (3) L SMA; (4) L ventral precentral; (5-8) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 219, but only the L SMA comparison is explicitly quantified |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T1 (n = 7) |
Covariate | TT T1 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 8 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) L STG/HG; (3) L SMA; (4) L ventral precentral; (5-8) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T2 (n = 7) |
Covariate | TT T2 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 8 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) L STG/HG; (3) L SMA; (4) L ventral precentral; (5-8) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | More activation in patients with more severe aphasia per TT |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent outcome (T2) TT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (the logic behind correlating activation changes and language outcome is unclear; TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L STG/HG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | Increase in activation for repetition was correlated with better aphasia outcome per TT |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T2 (n = 7) |
Covariate | Previous Δ (T2 vs T1) activation in L STG/HG |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (logically problematic because patients with less severe initial aphasia would also be expected to show little L temporal increase, but would not be expected to show R temporal recruitment) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R IFG; (2) R STG/HG; (3) R SMA; (4) R ventral precentral |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ R IFG ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | Patients with more increase in L STG/HG activation showed less activation of R hemisphere regions at T2 |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Aphasia with significant recovery over months to years (ADPASS > 70th percentile) |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 6 (plus 2 excluded: 1 unable to reliably describe performance post-scan; 1 due to head motion) |
Number of control participants | 37 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 20-56 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 1; females: 5) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 6; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 5-32 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | ADP |
Aphasia severity | ADPASS percentile range 73-99 |
Aphasia type | 3 anomic, 1 conduction, 1 recovered, 1 transcortical sensory |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Extents are reported in three dimensions |
Lesion location | 4 L MCA, 2 L ICA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Magnex Scientific 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 40 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (axial, perisylvian only) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (first level cross-correlation analysis unclear) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | N/A—no intersubject normalization |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming |
Control condition | Viewing nonsense drawings |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Insufficient data to assess the control activation pattern |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing nonsense drawings |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG and MFG; (2) L pSTG, AG and SMG; (3) R IFG and MFG; (4) R pSTG, AG and SMG; (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-4) individual anatomical images; activation quantified in terms of extent |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG ↓ LI (frontal) ↓ LI (temporal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing nonsense drawings |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Picture naming (outside scanner) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG and MFG; (2) L pSTG, AG and SMG; (3) R IFG and MFG; (4) R pSTG, AG and SMG; (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-4) individual anatomical images; activation quantified in terms of extent |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | AAT repetition ≥ 50 |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 23 |
Number of control participants | 11 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 56 ± 12 years, range 31-77 years; assume patient's age of 5.6 years is a typo for 56 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 15; females: 8) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 23; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (T1: ~2 weeks; T2: ~8 weeks) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | AAT, phonemic fluency |
Aphasia severity | T1: subcortical: TT median 8 errors, range 0-17 errors; frontal: TT median 21 errors, range 4-40 errors; temporal: TT median 39 errors, range 1-47 errors; T2: subcortical: TT median 1 error, range 0-14 errors; frontal: TT median 8 errors, range 0-34; temporal: TT median 34 errors, range 0-44 errors |
Aphasia type | T1: 6 Wernicke's, 5 Broca's, 5 residual aphasia, 4 anomic, 2 transcortical sensory, 1 conduction; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Extent and location |
Lesion extent | Range 4.3-154.3 cc (probably; units not stated) |
Lesion location | L MCA; 9 subcortical, 7 frontal, 7 temporal |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: ~2 weeks; T2: ~8 weeks |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Not stated |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 8 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | N/A—no intersubject normalization |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Noun repetition |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | L frontal and bilateral temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with subcortical damage (n = 9) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with frontal damage (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R mid temporal ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage T1 (n = 7) vs with subcortical damage T1 (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L posterior STG ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage T1 (n = 7) vs with frontal damage T1 (n = 7) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage T2 (n = 7) vs with subcortical damage T2 (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage T2 (n = 7) vs with frontal damage T2 (n = 7) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with subcortical damage T1 (n = 9) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with frontal damage T1 (n = 7) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage T1 (n = 7) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434; L IFG pars opercularis noted as different in text despite being significant in both groups |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with subcortical damage T2 (n = 9) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with frontal damage T2 (n = 7) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage T2 (n = 7) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 434 |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with subcortical or frontal damage and good recovery (n = 11) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on pp. 434-5 |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with subcortical or frontal damage and poor recovery (n = 5) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on pp. 434-5 |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with subcortical and frontal damage and good recovery T1 (n = 11) vs with subcortical and frontal damage and poor recovery T1 (n = 5) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 435 |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Noun repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with subcortical and frontal damage and good recovery T2 (n = 11) vs with subcortical and frontal damage and poor recovery T2 (n = 5) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars opercularis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L ventral precentral gyrus; (4) L Heschl's gyrus; (5) L temporal plane (posterior to HG, coded as posterior STG); (6) L posterior STG (coded as mid STG per Fig. 2); (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 435 |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Mild to moderate aphasia on TT; at least 50 out of 150 on AAT repetition |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 24 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (piracetam group: mean 57.4 ± 13.5 years; placebo group: mean 56.3 ± 10.0 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 13; females: 11) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 24; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (T1: ~2 weeks; T2: ~8 weeks) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | T1: piracetam group: TT 17.16 ± 14.31 errors; placebo group: TT 17.91 ± 15.47 errors; T2: piracetam group: TT 9.66 ± 12.62 errors; placebo group: TT 12.50 ± 16.88 errors |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Location only |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | 10 L frontal, 6 L subcortical, 8 L temporal |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—mixed |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment, ~2 weeks post onset; T2: post-treatment, ~8 weeks post onset |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | SLT, 1 hour/day, 5 days/week, 6 weeks; 12 patients received piracetam and 12 received placebo; note that the two groups are not directly compared in any imaging or behavioral analyses |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 8 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | N/A—no intersubject normalization |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word repetition |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | No control data are reported or cited, however the same task was used in several previous studies by this group |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated with pirecetam (n = 12) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L BA 44; (2) L BA 45; (3) L ventral PrCG; (4) L HG; (5) L BA 41 and 42; (6) L BA 22; (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word repetition vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated with placebo (n = 12) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L BA 44; (2) L BA 45; (3) L ventral PrCG; (4) L HG; (5) L BA 41 and 42; (6) L BA 22; (7) L SMA; (8-14) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual anatomical images |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | L IFG, possibly extending to neighboring regions |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 6 |
Number of control participants | 14 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (1 participant was reported in a previous case study) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (mean 47 years, range 32-72 years; control participants not age-matched) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 3; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 6; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 0.5-7.6 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB (except BDAE in 1 patient), reading pseudowords, word stem completion, verb generation, reading single words |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 74-97 (missing in 1 patient) |
Aphasia type | 3 anomic, 1 Broca's, 1 not stated, 1 recovered |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 10.7-117.5 cc |
Lesion location | L IFG, extending to neighboring areas in most cases |
Participants notes | Of the 14 controls, 6 were studied with PET and 8 with fMRI |
Modality | PET and fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens 961 EXACT HR; Siemens Vision 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (fMRI timing description is inconsistent) |
Design type | Mixed |
Total images acquired | PET: 10; fMRI: 384-768 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | 1 patient scanned on different PET scanner, and not scanned with fMRI; controls had different fMRI sequence to patients |
Language condition | Word stem completion (PET) |
Control condition | Rest (PET) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L IFG, L ITG, L anterior fusiform |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Word stem completion (fMRI) |
Control condition | Rest (fMRI) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L IFG, L intraparietal sulcus |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word stem completion (PET) vs rest (PET) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Unclear or not stated |
Software | not stated |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Correction for multiple comparisons unclear; there may be circularity in only correcting for the number of regions that seemed to show differences |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R IFG ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ L IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word stem completion (fMRI) vs rest (fMRI) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia (n = 5) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | not stated |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 1888 |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↓ L IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word stem completion (fMRI) vs rest (fMRI) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia (n = 5) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R IFG; (2) SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Not stated but seem to be functional |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Possibly circular because not clear how ROIs defined |
Findings | ↑ R IFG |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | L IFG, possibly extending to neighboring regions |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 |
Number of control participants | 14 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (mean 48.6 years; patients and controls not closely matched for age, unclear if difference significant) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 2; females: 6) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No (> 6 months; actual TPO not stated) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB or BDAE |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 66.5-89.0 in 6 participants, BDAE aphasia severity of 4 in 1 participant, no formal evaluation in 1 participant |
Aphasia type | 3 anomic, 3 transcortical motor, 1 Broca's, 1 not stated; most were Broca's or global acutely |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L IFG and operculum, extending to adjacent cortex and white matter in several cases |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Vision 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 1024 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (not described) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word stem completion (novel items) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Activation of language areas but also other areas; frontal activation is somewhat lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Word stem completion (novel items) |
Control condition | Word stem completion (repeated items) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | No whole brain analysis of this contrast, but somewhat lateralized in the sense that L but not R frontal areas showed a learning effect |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word stem completion (novel items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Covert task but overt data acquired separately; patients less accurate and slower than controls |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Unclear or not stated |
Software | not stated |
Voxelwise p | ~.001 (z > 3) |
Cluster extent | 45 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Monte Carlo analysis not described in detail; rather than fitting a HRF, the authors looked at the shape of the signal in the 8 volumes following each stimulus |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R dorsal precentral ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction |
Findings notes | Labels based on coordinates reported |
First level contrast | Word stem completion (novel items) vs word stem completion (repeated items) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | Covert task but overt data acquired separately; no interaction of group by practice for accuracy or RT |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 14 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L dorsal IFG; (2) L ventral IFG; (3) R MFG; (4) L anterior fusiform; (5) R anterior fusiform; (6) R posterior fusiform; (7) R lateral occipital; (8) R lateral cerebellum; (9) L SMA; (10) R dorsal IFG; (11) R posterior fusiform; (12) R lateral occipital; (13) R lingual; (14) L MTG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were active for the main effect of word stem completion (irrespective of practice) in either group and modulated by practice in that group |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group or the other; the L ROIs showed repetition suppression in controls but not in patients, and this difference is interpreted by the authors, but not supported statistically |
Findings | ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ L IFG ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | Labels based on coordinates reported |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 15 |
Number of control participants | 8 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 43-76 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 11; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 5-76 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | PPT (Dutch), British picture vocabulary scale, Action for Dysphasic Adults lexical decision battery, auditory maximal pairs (an offline phoneme discrimination test) |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated, but all 6 patients with pSTS damage had single word comprehension deficits acutely |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Extent and location |
Lesion extent | Range 0.5-14% of total brain volume |
Lesion location | 9 L but sparing pSTS, 6 L including pSTS |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR++/966) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 16 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Higher word rates |
Control condition | Lower word rates |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control activation is bilateral in primary auditory cortex and the lateral STG (Fig. 1, labels 1 and 2), but there is a left-lateralized activation in the pSTS (label 3); the scatter plots in Fig. 1 show activity-word rate curves for peak pSTS voxels in individual subjects; slopes were steeper in the left hemisphere (p < 0.05), however, the identification of these voxels is not described in sufficient detail (i.e. what was the search region?) |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Higher word rates vs lower word rates |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 555; a FWE-corrected SPM is reported of the relationship in the 6 patients with L pSTS damage (Fig. 2), however it is masked in a way that is not explained (see figure caption), and there is no direct comparison between patients with L pSTS damage and controls |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Higher word rates vs lower word rates |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with pSTS (n = 6) damage vs without pSTS damage (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 555; a FWE-corrected SPM is reported of the relationship in the 6 patients with L pSTS damage (Fig. 2), however it is masked in a way that is not explained (see figure caption), and there is no direct comparison between patients with L pSTS damage and patients with R pSTS damage |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Higher word rates vs lower word rates |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs control (n = 8) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R pSTS |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The peak voxel for the contrast in the R pSTS from each subject's individual analysis, but the search region is not stated |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | The controls and patients without pSTS damage were combined, however it is stated in the caption to Figure 2 that the patients with pSTS damage were significantly different to both |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Higher word rates vs lower word rates |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs aphasia without pSTS damage (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R pSTS |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The peak voxel for the contrast in the R pSTS from each subject's individual analysis, but the search region is not stated |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | The controls and patients without pSTS damage were combined, however it is stated in the caption to Figure 2 that the patients with pSTS damage were significantly different to both |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | Initial non-fluent aphasia due to anterior perisylvian lesion; subsequently recovered the ability to speak in sentences; patients were divided into those with and without damage to the IFG pars opercularis (POp+: n = 7; POp-: n = 7) |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 |
Number of control participants | 12 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (POp+: median 50 years, range 36-72 years; POp-: median 61 years, range 39-70 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 8; females: 6) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 14; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (POp+: median 39 months, range 19-134 months; POp-: median 17 months, range 6-240 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Type only |
Language evaluation | CAT, QPA |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | POp+: 4 non-fluent but not agrammatic, 2 agrammatic, 1 recovered; POp-: 4 non-fluent but not agrammatic, 3 recovered |
First stroke only? | No |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L frontal, occasionally extending into temporal |
Participants notes | 8 of 12 controls included in Blank et al. (2002) |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR++ (966)) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 15 (patients); 12 (controls) |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Much bilateral activation due to overt speech but pars opercularis and supratemporal plane L-lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Counting |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Extrasylvian; somewhat L-lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | Word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Small volume correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | No voxels survived FWE correction without SVC |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | Word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Small volume correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs without IFG POp damage (n = 7) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | Word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Small volume correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | Patients with L IFG POp damage showed numerically more signal in the R IFG POp |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs counting |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | Word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Small volume correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs counting |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | Word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Small volume correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs counting |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) vs without IFG POp damage (n = 7) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | Word rates not reported, but offline speech sample differed |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Small volume correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 with SVC in R pars opercularis |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) |
Covariate | Speech rate during scan |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R IFG pars opercularis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Defined by flipping L IFG pars opercularis activation in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 7) |
Covariate | Speech rate during scan |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R IFG pars opercularis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Defined by flipping L IFG pars opercularis activation in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 7) |
Covariate | Four different QPA measures |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R IFG pars opercularis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Defined by flipping L IFG pars opercularis activation in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | French |
Inclusion criteria | No severe aphasia; no leukoaraiosis |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 |
Number of control participants | 6 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 58.4 ± 11.9 years, range 37-73 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 7; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No* (moderate limitation) (T1: 58 ± 35 days, range 11-113 days; T2: 11.7 ± 1.6 months, range 320-460 days; T1 varies considerably from early to late subacute) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | Not stated |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | T1: some prominent symptoms are listed for each patient; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | 4 L subcortical, 2 L prerolandic, 2 L postrolandic |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: 58 ± 35 days, range 11-113 days; T2: 11.7 ± 1.6 months, range 320-460 days; T1 varies considerably from early to late subacute |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Not stated |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens ECAT HR+) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 6 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word generation |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral fronto-temporal and some other regions per text |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Nature of inclusive masks unclear |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Based on Figure 2 |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ word generation accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 100 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Nature of inclusive masks unclear |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L occipital ↓ L hippocampus/MTL ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R occipital |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | Lesion in vicinity of L STG; no extensive frontal damage; no inferior temporal damage; able to perform tasks |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 9 |
Number of control participants | 18 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (median 58 years, range 39-72 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 8; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 9; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 45 months, range 14-145 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | Subtests from CAT, subtests from PALPA, Action for dysphasic adults, TROG, PPT |
Aphasia severity | Mild |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | Lesion in vicinity of L STG; no extensive frontal damage; no inferior temporal damage |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens HR++ 966) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 16 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Syllable count decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | The control data provided also include the noise vocoded conditions; only ventral temporal activations are shown, which are L-lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs syllable count decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control (clear speech) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | Interaction of group by task not reported for accuracy |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Small volume correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 with SVC in fusiform gyri, temporal poles, L IFG, L orbitofrontal and L SFG |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs syllable count decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Semantic decision accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Small volume correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 with SVC in fusiform gyri, temporal poles, L IFG, L orbitofrontal and L SFG |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Fixed effects; this analysis is not clearly described |
Findings | ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | Patients who were more accurate had more activity in R anterior fusiform gyrus |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs syllable count decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control (clear speech) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | Interaction of group by task not reported for accuracy |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L fusiform gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Probabilistic brain atlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs syllable count decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control (noise vocoded) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, but attempt made |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | Patients were more accurate on semantic decisions than syllable decisions, whereas controls were less accurate on noise vocoded semantic decisions than clear syllable decisions (which were the baseline for this analysis) |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L fusiform gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Probabilistic brain atlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | This analysis suggests that the difference between groups in the L fusiform gyrus disappears when the controls perform a semantic task that is similarly challenging |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Global aphasia in the first three months; some improvement of comprehension within 6-12 months |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 7 |
Number of control participants | 14 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 29-67 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 6; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 7; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 6 months-4 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AABT, AAT |
Aphasia severity | TT percentile range 28-63 |
Aphasia type | 3 global, 2 Broca's, 2 unclassifiable; all had been global initially |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips ACS NT Gyroscan 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (insufficient blocks per experimental condition (3) because blocks were too long (44 s)) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 198 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | N/A—no intersubject normalization |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Phonetic decision and lexical decision (conjunction) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Tasks were matched in controls, but no statistics reported for patients |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L-lateralized frontal activation, as well as temporal and parietal to a lesser extent |
Contrast notes | Conjunction of baseline conditions not described in sufficient detail |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs phonetic decision and lexical decision (conjunction) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Relative performance on language and control tasks unclear |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Conjunction analyses not clearly described; in two patients, a different conjunction was used (lexical decision vs phonetic decision & semantic decision vs phonetic decision) |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | LI > 0 in 12 out of 14 controls and 5 out of 7 patients; no significant difference |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 17 |
Number of control participants | 18 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 62 ± 2.7 SEM years, range 34-75 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 12; females: 5) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 17; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 4-125 months; aphasia with temporal damage (n=8) mean 41 months; aphasia without temporal damage (n=9) mean 48 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | CAT |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | No (Siemens 1.5 Tesla; model not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (the calculated duration of the stimuli, the calculated duration of the acquisitions, and the stated duration of the acquisitions yield three different numbers) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 460 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to narrative speech |
Control condition | Listening to reversed speech |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Bilateral (L > R) temporal, L IFG and L dorsal precentral |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia without temporal lobe damage (n = 9) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction and additional arbitrary cluster correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | 5 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L dorsal precentral ↓ R somato-motor |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal lobe damage (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction and additional arbitrary cluster correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | 5 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L posterior STS ↓ L mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal lobe damage (n = 8) vs without temporal lobe damage (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction and additional arbitrary cluster correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | 5 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia without temporal lobe damage (n = 9) |
Covariate | Sentence comprehension (CAT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction and additional arbitrary cluster correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | 5 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Conjunction with main effect of story comprehension (details hard to follow); this was a multiple regression also involving patients with temporal lobe damage |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STS ↑ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | Patients with better sentence comprehension had more activation in the L posterior STS and R mid STS |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal lobe damage (n = 8) |
Covariate | Sentence comprehension (CAT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction and additional arbitrary cluster correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | 5 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Conjunction with main effect of story comprehension (details hard to follow); this was a multiple regression also involving patients without temporal lobe damage |
Findings | ↑ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | Patients with better sentence comprehension had more activation in the R mid STS |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage (n = 8) vs without temporal damage (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations were computed between activity in each voxel, and the sentence comprehension measure from the CAT, and were compared between the two aphasia groups, in regions with a main effect of story comprehension. The voxelwise threshold was p < .001, uncorrected for multiple comparisons. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Activity in the L posterior STS was positively correlated with sentence comprehension in patients without temporal lobe damage, but not in patients with temporal lobe damage |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia without temporal damage (n = 9) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations were computed between activity in each voxel, and post-scan story recall, and were compared between patients without temporal damage and controls, in regions with a main effect of story comprehension. The threshold was p < 0.05 corrected, plus a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations were computed between activity in each voxel, and post-scan story recall, and were compared between patients with temporal damage and controls, in regions with a main effect of story comprehension. The threshold was p < 0.05 corrected, plus a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage (n = 8) vs without temporal damage (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations were computed between activity in each voxel, and post-scan story recall, and were compared between the two aphasia groups, in regions with a main effect of story comprehension. The threshold was p < 0.05 corrected, plus a minimum cluster size of 5 voxels. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | French |
Inclusion criteria | Subcortical stroke; no severe aphasia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 7 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 52.4 ± 13 years, range 31-69 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 7; females: 0) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 7; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No* (moderate limitation) (T1: mean 53 ± 35 days, range 11-108 days; T2: mean 12.2 ± 1.4 months; T1 varies considerably from early to late subacute) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Type only |
Language evaluation | Montreal-Toulouse Aphasia Battery |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | T1: 2 Broca's, 2 transcortical sensory, 1 anomic, 1 transcortical motor, 1 Wernicke's; T2: 4 recovered, 1 anomic, 1 transcortical motor; 1 transcortical sensory |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | 5 L non-thalamic subcortical, 2 L thalamic |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: mean 53 ± 35 days, range 11-108 days; T2: mean 12.2 ± 1.4 months; T1 varies considerably from early to late subacute |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Not stated |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR+) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 6 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed; minimal due to lesions being small and subcortical) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word generation |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Time post onset |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No significant correlation between time post onset and accuracy |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital |
Findings notes | More activity with longer time post onset; based on coordinates in Table 3a |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Word generation accuracy T1 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L precuneus ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ R insula ↑ R posterior STG |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 3b |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 100 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Description of masking unclear, but seems to be inclusively masked with T1, which seems inappropriate |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L posterior STG ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 2 |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ word generation accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 20 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 3c |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | L IFG, possibly extending to neighboring regions |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 |
Number of control participants | 14 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (re-analysis of data from Blasi et al. (2002)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (mean 48.6 years; patients and controls not closely matched for age, unclear if difference significant) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 2; females: 6) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No (> 6 months; actual TPO not stated) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB or BDAE |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 66.5-89.0 in 6 participants, BDAE aphasia severity of 4 in 1 participant, no formal evaluation in 1 participant |
Aphasia type | 3 anomic, 3 transcortical motor, 1 Broca's, 1 not stated; most were Broca's or global acutely |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L IFG and operculum, extending to adjacent cortex and white matter in several cases |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Vision 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 1024 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word stem completion (novel items) |
Control condition | Word stem completion (repeated items) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | No whole brain analysis of this contrast, but somewhat lateralized in the sense that L but not R frontal areas showed a learning effect |
Contrast notes | The only contrast analyzed in this paper is the "learning" contrast which corresponds to contrast 2 in Blasi et al. (2002) |
First level contrast | Word stem completion (novel items) vs word stem completion (repeated items) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | Covert task but overt data acquired separately; no interaction of group by practice for accuracy or RT |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Cerebellum |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | not stated |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 174; Monte Carlo-based thresholding not described; rather than fitting a HRF, the authors looked at the shape of the signal in the 8 volumes following each stimulus |
Findings | ↑ L cerebellum ↓ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word stem completion (novel items) vs word stem completion (repeated items) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | Covert task but overt data acquired separately; no interaction of group by practice for accuracy or RT |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L cerebellum |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | L cerebellar region with a learning effect in the patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group; rather than fitting a HRF, the authors looked at the shape of the signal in the 8 volumes following each stimulus |
Findings | ↑ L cerebellum |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 24 |
Number of control participants | 11 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 32-85 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 18; females: 6) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 24; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No (mean 32 months, range 2-204 months; combines subacute and chronic patients) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | CAT (missing in two participants) |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | 6 L but no temporal damage, 9 L temporal damage excluding anterior temporal cortex, 9 L temporal damage including anterior temporal cortex |
Participants notes | Results of control participants previously reported in Crinion et al. (2003) |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR++/966 (16 patients and all controls) or GE Advance (8 patients)) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 12-16 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | two different scanners used for patients, but not for controls |
Language condition | Listening to narrative speech |
Control condition | Listening to reversed speech |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | 11 participants; L-lateralized posterior temporal, bilateral anterior temporal, no frontal |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia without temporal lobe damage (n = 6) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all included patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal lobe damage (n = 18) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all included patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with no temporal damage (excluding 1 with missing behavioral data and 1 outlier) or posterior temporal damage sparing anterior temporal cortex (n = 13) |
Covariate | Auditory sentence comprehension (CAT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activation in the control group |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Same result obtained with or without excluding one outlier; two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses |
Findings | ↑ L anterior temporal |
Findings notes | More activity in patients with better auditory sentence comprehension |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with no temporal damage (excluding 1 with missing behavioral data and 1 outlier) or posterior temporal damage sparing anterior temporal cortex (n = 13) |
Covariate | Time post onset |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activation in the control group |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage excluding anterior temporal cortex (n = 9) vs with no temporal lobe damage (excluding 1 with missing behavioral data and 1 outlier) (n = 4) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activation in the control group |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses |
Findings | ↓ L anterior temporal |
Findings notes | Patients with posterior temporal damage had less signal change |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with temporal damage excluding anterior temporal cortex (n = 9) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activation in the control group |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Circular because ROI defined in one group; two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses |
Findings | ↓ L anterior temporal |
Findings notes | Large difference 2.7 ± 0.8 (patients) vs 6.3 ± 1.4 (controls) makes finding suggestive even in light of the circularity |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with no temporal damage (excluding 1 with missing behavioral data and 1 outlier) or posterior temporal damage sparing anterior temporal cortex (n = 13) |
Covariate | Auditory single word comprehension (CAT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activation in the control group |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Two other ROIs are described in the methods, but never used in any analyses |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | R = 0.39; p > 0.1; seems to be a clear trend so lack of significance may reflect only lack of power |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | MCA; age < 70 years; able to distinguish forward vs backward speech outside the scanner; no pronounced small vessel disease |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 (plus 4 excluded: 1 health problems; 1 scanner noise; 2 did not tolerate fMRI) |
Number of control participants | 14 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 51.9 ± 14.2 years, range 16-68 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 12; left: 1; other: 1) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (T1 acute: mean 1.8 days, range 0-4 days; T2 subacute: mean 12.1 days, range 3-16 days; T3 chronic: mean 321 days, range 102-513 days) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AABT, AAT including TT, analysis of spontaneous speech, CETI, Language Recovery Score (LRS) derived from all these measures plus in-scanner task performance |
Aphasia severity | T1: LRS mean 0.44, range 0.11-0.81; 1 mild, 1 mild-moderate, 7 moderate, 3 moderate-severe, 2 severe per AAT; T2: LRS mean 0.71, range 0.33-0.92; 2 recovered, 2 recovered-mild, 2 mild, 3 mild-moderate, 3 moderate, 2 severe per AAT; T3: LRS mean 0.91, range 0.66-1.00; 8 recovered, 2 recovered-mild, 3 mild, 1 moderate per AAT |
Aphasia type | T1: 9 non-fluent, 5 fluent; T2: not stated; T3: 6 recovered, 4 minimal language impairment, 3 anomic, 1 global |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA; 4 frontal (2 extending to temporoparietal); 5 temporoparietal (2 extending to subcortical); 4 striatocapsular (2 extending to cortical); 1 frontoparietal |
Participants notes | 198 patients with aphasia were screened |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1 acute: mean 1.8 days, range 0-4 days; T2 subacute: mean 12.1 days, range 3-16 days; T3 chronic: mean 321 days, range 102-513 days |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Standard SLT throughout the observation period including at least 3 weeks inpatient |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 660 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment |
Control condition | Listening to reversed speech |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Reported accuracy combines the two conditions in a way that is not explained |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L temporal and L > R frontal |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | R IFG/insula activation noted to survive FWE correction at p < .05 |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | Threshold was lowered to reveal the R frontal change in activation |
Findings | ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R occipital |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R insula |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L insula ↓ L posterior MTG ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R insula |
Findings notes | L STG in table is actually MTG based on coordinates |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | Threshold was lowered to reveal L IFG |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L insula ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Language recovery score T1 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Language recovery score T2 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 |
Covariate | Language recovery score T3 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | % change in language recovery score |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | % change in language recovery score |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | % change in language recovery score |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | Some other ROIs also significant prior to correction for multiple comparisons; n.b. performance confound |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | Some other ROIs also significant prior to correction for multiple comparisons; n.b. performance confound |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L posterior MTG |
Findings notes | Some other ROIs also significant prior to correction for multiple comparisons; n.b. performance confound |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | ↓ L posterior MTG ↓ R IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | R IFG difference described in text but not table |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy combines language and control conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars triangularis; (3) L MTG; (4) R insula; (5) R IFG pars triangularis; (6) R SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxels of overall activation map based on all three time points in patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 11 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (median 51.0 years, range 19-66 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 7; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 11; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (median 32 months; range 6-480 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT, study-specific picture naming test with 150 items |
Aphasia severity | 6 moderate, 4 mild, 1 severe |
Aphasia type | 7 Broca's, 2 Wernicke's, 1 global, 1 unclassified |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 31.0-236.0 cc |
Lesion location | L |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~2 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | CIAT, 3 hours/day, 5 days/week, 2 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Intera 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 160 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (trained items) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (untrained items) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ picture naming (trained items) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Picture naming score (trained items) increased from 51.7 ± 24.8 to 78.8 ± 22.1, which was statistically significant (p < 0.0001) |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional area of slow wave activity determined with MEG; (2) right hemisphere homotopic to lesion; (3) right hemisphere homotopic to slow wave area; (4) remainder of left hemisphere; for one patient, maximal slow wave activity was in the right hemisphere and it is not clear how this was handled |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The dependent measure was the number of voxels in each ROI exceeding certain thresholds that differed across subjects depending on their strength of activation; it appears that increases and decreases may have been summed, though the description is hard to follow |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | 2 of the 11 patients were classified as outliers and excluded from analyses, however no plots are provided to justify their status as outliers |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Improved picture naming of trained items was correlated with increased signal in 3 of the 4 ROIs, the exception being the right hemisphere ROI homotopic to the slow wave area; after removing the two outliers, only the correlation in the left hemisphere area of slow wave activity remained significant |
First level contrast | Picture naming (untrained items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ picture naming (untrained items) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Picture naming score (untrained items) increased from 54.0 ± 24.3 to 70.5 ± 26.7, which was statistically significant (p= 0.002) |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional area of slow wave activity determined with MEG; (2) right hemisphere homotopic to lesion; (3) right hemisphere homotopic to slow wave area; (4) remainder of left hemisphere; for one patient, maximal slow wave activity was in the right hemisphere and it is not clear how this was handled |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The dependent measure was the number of voxels in each ROI exceeding certain thresholds that differed across subjects depending on their strength of activation; it appears that increases and decreases may have been summed, though the description is hard to follow |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | 2 of the 11 patients were classified as outliers and excluded from analyses, however no plots are provided to justify their status as outliers |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Improved picture naming of untrained items was correlated with increased signal in all 4 ROIs; after removing the two outliers, none of the correlations remained significant |
Language | French |
Inclusion criteria | Naming deficit; good comprehension |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 10 |
Number of control participants | 20 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (mean 53.8 ± 14.7 years; controls were younger) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 6; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 10; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 7-102 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | Montreal-Toulouse Aphasia Battery |
Aphasia severity | Mild (but had initially been severe) |
Aphasia type | 4 anomic, 3 conduction, 2 Broca's, 1 AoS |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 29.9-195.2 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~4 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Lexical training, 15 minutes/day, 5 days/week, 4 weeks; the control group were trained to relearn foreign words that they had learned in school but since mostly forgotten |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens ECAT HR+) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 6 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (native in patients; relearned foreign in controls) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Presumably only the relearned foreign condition was used in controls (not the native condition), but this is not stated explicitly |
First level contrast | Picture naming (native in patients; relearned foreign in controls) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | (Aphasia T2 vs T1) vs (control T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, but attempt made |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Relearned foreign language was an attempt to equate to recovery in patients; still, patients improved less than controls, as shown by a significant interaction of group by time (p < .0001) |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 30 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Nature of control contrast not clear; negative tail of contrast was masked to exclude lesioned areas, but the mask may have been more extensive than that |
Findings | ↑ L orbitofrontal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (native in patients; relearned foreign in controls) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ picture naming accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 30 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Nature of control contrast not clear |
Findings | ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L intraparietal sulcus ↓ L precuneus ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ R dorsal precentral ↓ R precuneus |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Main deficits in production rather than comprehension |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 16 (plus 8 excluded: 5 completed only one of the two sessions; 3 unable to perform the tasks) |
Number of control participants | 8 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 58.3 years; range 42-73 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 12; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 16; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No (> 12 months; actual TPO not stated) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT, two subtests of ANELT |
Aphasia severity | TT range 5-50 |
Aphasia type | 7 anomic, 7 Broca's, 2 global; it was an inclusion criterion that the main deficits were in production |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~2 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | CIAT, 3 hours/day, 10 days |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Vision plus 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (minor discrepancies in description of timing) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 134 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Reading words silently |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Appears to be somewhat L-lateralized frontal, but not well visualized |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Word stem completion |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral frontal; other regions not well visualized |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Reading words silently vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | R hemisphere |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Mixed** (major limitation) |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.7 |
Voxelwise p | R IFG/R insula ROI: .005; elsewhere: .001 |
Cluster extent | R IFG/R insula ROI: 0.108 cc; elsewhere: none |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word stem completion vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | R hemisphere |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Mixed** (major limitation) |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.7 |
Voxelwise p | R IFG/R insula ROI: .005; elsewhere: .001 |
Cluster extent | R IFG/R insula ROI: 0.108 cc; elsewhere: none |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R dorsal precentral |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Reading words silently vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | R hemisphere |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.7 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | Nature of thresholding not entirely clear, so coded according to best guess |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R posterior MTG |
Findings notes | Increased activity correlated with more behavioral improvement |
First level contrast | Word stem completion vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | R hemisphere |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.7 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | Nature of thresholding not entirely clear, so coded according to best guess |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula |
Findings notes | Increased activity correlated with more behavioral improvement |
First level contrast | Reading words silently vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | R hemisphere |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Mixed** (major limitation) |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.7 |
Voxelwise p | R IFG/R insula ROI: .005; elsewhere: .001 |
Cluster extent | R IFG/R insula ROI: 0.108 cc; elsewhere: none |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word stem completion vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | R hemisphere |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Mixed** (major limitation) |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.7 |
Voxelwise p | R IFG/R insula ROI: .005; elsewhere: .001 |
Cluster extent | R IFG/R insula ROI: 0.108 cc; elsewhere: none |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Reading words silently vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L IFG/insula or L perilesional |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak activations in individual patients in L IFG/insula or L perilesional regions (somewhat unclear) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word stem completion vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L IFG/insula or L perilesional |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak activations in individual patients in L IFG/insula or L perilesional regions (somewhat unclear) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Reading words silently vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R IFG/insula; (2) R precentral; (3) R MTG; (4) L IFG/insula or L perilesional |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions where T1 activation was correlated with subsequent improvement, along with the previously defined left hemisphere ROI |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because functional ROIs based on related contrast on same data |
Findings | ↓ R posterior MTG |
Findings notes | Decreased activity over time correlated with more behavioral improvement |
First level contrast | Word stem completion vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ overall language measure (composite measure of AAT spontaneous speech, token test, ANELT auditory comprehensibility, ANELT semantic comprehensibility) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1, 2) two clusters within R IFG/insula ROI; (3) L IFG/insula or L perilesional |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions where T1 activation was correlated with subsequent improvement, along with the previously defined left hemisphere ROI |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because functional ROIs based on related contrast on same data |
Findings | ↓ R IFG ↓ R insula |
Findings notes | Decreased activity over time correlated with more behavioral improvement |
Language | French |
Inclusion criteria | Only part of L MCA; able to perform word generation; no severe aphasia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 13 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (7 out of 13 patients appear to represent the same data reported in de Boissezon et al. (2005)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 31.2-74.2 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 12; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 13; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No* (moderate limitation) (T1: mean 64 ± 32 days; T2: mean 11.8 ± 1.4 months; T1 varies considerably from early to late subacute) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | Montreal-Toulouse Aphasia Battery |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | T1: 3 transcortical motor, 2 anomic, 2 Broca's, 2 transcortical sensory, 2 Wernicke's, 1 conduction, 1 agrammatic; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 0.9-43.4 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA (7 subcortical, 6 cortical) |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: mean 64 ± 32 days; T2: mean 11.8 ± 1.4 months; T1 varies considerably from early to late subacute |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Community SLT; 45 minutes/day, 1-3 days/week |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR+) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 6 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word generation |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Control data in Cardebat et al. (2003); bilateral fronto-temporal and some other regions per text |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with "good recovery" (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (the "good recovery" group showed more improvement than the "poor recovery" group in terms of accuracy on the task, but the distinction was not borne out in behavioral data more generally) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | P = 0.07 |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 100 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Contrast may not have included resting condition; inappropriate masking |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R occipital ↑ R thalamus ↑ R basal ganglia ↓ L cerebellum |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 5 |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with "poor recovery" (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (the "poor recovery" group showed less improvement than the "good recovery" group in terms of accuracy on the task, but the distinction was not borne out in behavioral data more generally) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 100 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Contrast may not have included resting condition; inappropriate masking |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R cerebellum ↓ R basal ganglia |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Word generation accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 100 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Each patient's two sessions may be entered into the model without accounting for the dependence between them |
Findings | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital ↓ L cerebellum |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 11 |
Number of control participants | 10 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 58.8 ± 14.7 years, range 33-78 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 6; females: 5) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 10-101 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB; BNT |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 31.8-91.5 |
Aphasia type | 6 anomic, 4 Broca's, 1 transcortical motor; alternatively: 6 fluent, 5 non-fluent |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 3.0-342.2 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | No (not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (timing of picture presentation not clearly explained) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 120 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Picture naming (correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing scrambled images |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control data in Fridriksson et al. (2007); motor activations are prominent; there is some L frontal activation but little temporal activation in either hemisphere |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (phonemic paraphasias) |
Control condition | Picture naming (correct trials) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | N/A |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | N/A |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | N/A |
Control activation notes | Control data N/A because controls do not typically make errors |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (semantic paraphasias) |
Control condition | Picture naming (correct trials) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | N/A |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | N/A |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | N/A |
Control activation notes | Control data N/A because controls do not typically make errors |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL (FEAT 5.4) |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (phonemic paraphasias) vs picture naming (correct trials) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional performance-defined conditions |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL (FEAT 5.4) |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L occipital |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (semantic paraphasias) vs picture naming (correct trials) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional performance-defined conditions |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL (FEAT 5.4) |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R occipital |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Picture naming accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R IFG/insula; (2) R motor/premotor; (3) R SMA; (4) R inferior parietal; (5) R superior temporal |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions activated for picture naming vs viewing scrambled images in aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula |
Findings notes | R IFG showed more activation in patients who produced more correct responses |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Moderate to severe anomia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 |
Number of control participants | 9 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 34-67 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 1.8-6.9 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | 6 moderate-severe, 2 severe |
Aphasia type | 7 Broca's, 1 global |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~2 weeks later; T3: 8 months after the end of treatment |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Intensive anomia training; 3 hours/day; 2 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Intera 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (total images acquired not stated) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | probably ~360, but not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (trained items) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Table of coordinates only |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (untrained items) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Table of coordinates only |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent outcome (T2) picture naming of trained items outside the scanner |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (the logic behind correlating activation changes and language outcome is unclear) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Mixed** (major limitation) |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .05, but at least one voxel in the cluster had to be p < .001 |
Cluster extent | 0.270 cc |
Statistical details | There was an exclusive mask based on activation changes for untrained pictures, but it is unclear what the behavioral covariate was for the mask generation, nor were the regions in the mask reported |
Findings | ↑ L occipital ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R precuneus ↑ R occipital ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R hippocampus/MTL |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (untrained items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent outcome (T3) picture naming of trained items outside the scanner |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (the logic behind correlating activation changes and language outcome is unclear) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Mixed** (major limitation) |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | .05, but at least one voxel in the cluster had to be p < .001 |
Cluster extent | 0.270 cc |
Statistical details | There was an exclusive mask based on activation changes for untrained pictures, but it is unclear what the behavioral covariate was for the mask generation, nor were the regions in the mask reported |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R inferior parietal lobule ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R basal ganglia |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 12 |
Number of control participants | 12 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (mean 49 + 14 years, range 30-71 years; controls were younger) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 9; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No (mean 1.9 ± 1.4 years, range 0.2-3.7 years; one non-chronic patient is included) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | 3 global, 3 Wernicke's, 2 amnestic, 2 Broca's, 2 unclassified |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA, with greatest overlap in the posterior STG |
Participants notes | 15 controls were scanned but 3 were randomly excluded to match group sizes for jICA. |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens HR+) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 9 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Lexical decision (words vs pseudowords) |
Control condition | Lexical decision (words vs reversed foreign words) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | The contrast activated a ventral part of the L IFG, along with L anterior cingulate and L DLPFC |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Lexical decision (words vs pseudowords) vs lexical decision (words vs reversed foreign words) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 0.64 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | Activation is 1105 voxels (> 8 cc) so quite convincing, but when the contrast was examined in the patient group, this region was not activated. |
First level contrast | Lexical decision (words vs pseudowords) vs lexical decision (words vs reversed foreign words) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Joint ICA was performed on structural and functional contrast images using FIT 1.1b. Only 1 of the 8 components differed between groups in its loadings and was interpretable. The structural part of this component related to the patients' lesions. The functional part was thresholded at voxelwise p < .001 (CDT), arbitrary minimum cluster extent = 0.64 cc. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | The component that differed between groups showed more activation for patients than controls in the L anterior temporal lobe, L cerebellum, R posterior STG, R anterior temporal lobe, R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus, R cerebellum, and R brainstem, and less activation in patients than controls in the L IFG, L anterior temporal lobe, L occipital lobe, L anterior cingulate, L cerebellum, L thalamus, and R IFG. |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | Comprehension deficit per CAT and TROG (1 patient did not meet this criterion); anterolateral superior temporal cortex spared |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 16 (plus 8 excluded: lesions involved L anterolateral superior temporal cortex) |
Number of control participants | 11 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (reanalysis of subset of dataset from Crinion et al. (2006)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (mean 65.8 ± 2.0 SEM years; controls were younger) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 5) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 16; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No (mean 28.8 ± 9.2 months SEM; minimum time post onset not reported, but some patients in Crinion et al. (2006) were subacute) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | CAT, TROG |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Patients with positive anterior temporal interconnectivity: mean 93.3 ± 24.0 cc; patients with negative anterior temporal interconnectivity: mean 96.1 ± 27.6 cc |
Lesion location | L not including anterolateral superior temporal cortex; maximal overlap in posterior superior temporal cortex |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR++/966 (10 patients and all controls) or GE Advance (6 patients)) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 12-16 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | two different scanners used for patients, but not for controls |
Language condition | Listening to narrative speech |
Control condition | Listening to reversed speech |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | 11 participants; L-lateralized posterior temporal, bilateral anterior temporal, no frontal |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Somewhat circular because ROIs were defined only in regions where controls showed significant connectivity (even though ROIs were anatomical) |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | L IFG pars triangularis almost reached significance (p = .053) for more activation in patients |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Auditory sentence comprehension |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L anterior temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Written sentence comprehension |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Auditory single word comprehension |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | L anterior temporal p = .08 |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Auditory syntactic comprehension |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | L anterior temporal p = .09 |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Connectivity between L and R ATL |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) R anterior superior temporal cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Time post onset |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L anterior superior temporal cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L anterior superior temporal cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with positive anterior temporal interconnectivity (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Somewhat circular because ROIs were defined only in regions where controls showed significant connectivity (even though ROIs were anatomical); excluded 3 patients with L IFG damage |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with negative anterior temporal interconnectivity (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Somewhat circular because ROIs were defined only in regions where controls showed significant connectivity (even though ROIs were anatomical); excluded 1 patient with L IFG damage |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with positive anterior temporal interconnectivity (n = 8) vs with negative anterior temporal interconnectivity (n = 8) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior superior temporal cortex; (2) L basal temporal language area; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4-6) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs were defined anatomically in regions that were functionally connected with L anterior superior temporal cortex in controls (1-4) or homotopic to these (5-6) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Excluded 4 patients with L IFG damage |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion status of each voxel |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | VLSM with FDR correction was used to identify any regions in which damage was predictive of L anterior temporal activation. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | Cantonese |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 7 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 63 ± 10 years, range 56-79 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 2) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 7; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 17 ± 8 months, range 8-28 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | Cantonese Aphasia Battery (modified WAB) |
Aphasia severity | 5 patients had AQ > 75, 2 had AQ < 30 |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Location only |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | 3 L MCA, 2 L frontal, 2 L basal ganglia |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~10 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Acupuncture, 3 sessions/week, 8 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | No (not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (inconsistent information regarding timing) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 90? |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Answering questions from Cantonese Aphasia Battery |
Control condition | Visual decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Answering questions from Cantonese Aphasia Battery vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ WAB AQ |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Unclear or not stated |
Software | SPM2 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Stated to be corrected p < 0.05, but the nature of correction is not described; it is not entirely clear whether the functional measure was the difference between T1 and T2 (we assume it is); it is also not clear whether or not 2 patients with low AQ were excluded (we assume not) |
Findings | ↑ L posterior MTG |
Findings notes | Finding based on table; additional small activations are shown in figure but not table |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 19 (plus 7 excluded: 6 for making fewer than 5 correct responses in one or more sessions; 1 for excessive head motion) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes ("several" patients overlapped with those reported by Fridriksson et al. (2009, 2010)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 59.7 ± 12.3 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 12; females: 14) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (> 8 months; actual TPO not stated) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB |
Aphasia severity | AQ mean 60.4 ± 25.6 (including excluded patients) |
Aphasia type | 11 anomic, 10 Broca's, 3 conduction, 1 transcortical motor, 1 Wernicke's (including excluded patients) |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | Demographic data includes excluded patients |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment/~4 weeks later; note that there were two separate sessions per time point, as well as another two sessions midway through treatment that are not analyzed in this paper |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Anomia treatment using a cueing hierarchy, 3 hours/day, 5 days/week, 2 weeks, with a 1-week gap between the two weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (timing of stimuli within the silent periods is unclear) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 120 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Picture naming (correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing abstract pictures |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control data in Fridriksson et al. (2007); motor activations are prominent; there is some L frontal activation but little temporal activation in either hemisphere. |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ picture naming accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 4.1 |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L precuneus |
Findings notes | Activated regions were on the borders on the lesion distribution in the 19 included patients |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 15 |
Number of control participants | 9 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 61.9 years, range 41-81 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (males: 7; females: 8; not stated for controls) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 29.7 months, > 6 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB |
Aphasia severity | AQ mean 77.1, range 47.1-93.7 |
Aphasia type | 10 anomic, 3 Broca's, 2 conduction |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (exact timing of picture presentation not specified) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 120 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Picture naming (correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing abstract pictures |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | L-lateralized frontal and temporal activations, but also bilateral visual, motor and auditory |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Picture naming accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 4.1 |
Voxelwise p | ~.02 (z > 2) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate |
Findings notes | Greater activation was associated with better picture naming; L IFG pars orbitalis activation classified as middle frontal gyrus in the paper, but coordinates suggest otherwise |
First level contrast | Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Picture naming accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | A single ROI comprising 3 regions where activation in patients was correlated with picture naming accuracy: the L IFG pars orbitalis, occipital lobe, and anterior cingulate |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on SPM analysis 1 |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | The purpose of this analysis was to determine whether these regions were recruited in the patients with better naming, or not activated in the patients with worse naming, relative to the control mean |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Patients with better naming showed greater activation than controls, while the patients with poorer naming showed less activation than controls. |
First level contrast | Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion status of each voxel |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | VLSM was used to identify any regions in which damage was predictive of activation in the regions identified in SPM analysis 1, considered as a single ROI. There was no correction for multiple comparisons, and the analysis is appropriately presented as exploratory. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Only in the L IFG pars opercularis was damage predictive of reduced activation in the potentially compensatory network. |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | Lesion in vicinity of L STG; no extensive frontal damage; no inferior temporal damage; able to perform tasks |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 9 |
Number of control participants | 18 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (additional analysis of same dataset as Sharp et al. (2004)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (median 58 years, range 39-72 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 8; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 9; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 45 months, range 14-145 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | Subtests from CAT, subtests from PALPA, Action for dysphasic adults, TROG, PPT |
Aphasia severity | Mild |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | Lesion in vicinity of L STG; no extensive frontal damage; no inferior temporal damage |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens HR++ 966) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 16 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision (clear in patients; average of clear and noise vocoded in controls) |
Control condition | Syllable count decision (clear in patients; average of clear and noise vocoded in controls) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Significant differences per Sharp et al. (2004) |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Not stated exactly what contrast was used in controls |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision (clear in patients; average of clear and noise vocoded in controls) vs syllable count decision (clear in patients; average of clear and noise vocoded in controls) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, but attempt made |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy and RT were not significantly different for the semantic task; statistics are not reported for the syllable counting task, but the data provided suggest that accuracy was probably not matched, while RT probably was |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 12 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Functional connectivity between pairs of spared nodes of the L hemisphere semantic network and R hemisphere homotopic regions: (1) L SFG-L AG; (2) L SFG-L IFG; (3) L SFG-L IT; (4) L AG-L IFG; (5) L AG-L IT; (6) L IFG-L IT; (7-12) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Partial correlations between nodes |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Patients showed greater connectivity between L SFG and L AG than controls while performing the semantic task; this was not the case for the syllable counting task, however connectivity during performance of the two tasks was not compared directly |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Agrammatic |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 6 |
Number of control participants | 12 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 54 years, range 38-66 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 6; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 6-146 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, NAVS, narrative language sample |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 66.8-85.0 |
Aphasia type | All agrammatic; per WAB scores provided: 3 Broca's, 3 unclassified |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | 5 L MCA, 1 R MCA with aphasia |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, 9-15 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Treatment of underlying forms |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (total images acquired not stated) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Auditory sentence-picture matching (all three sentence types) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Auditory sentence-picture matching (all three sentence types) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 18 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L BA 7; (2) L BA 9; (3) L BA 13; (4) L BA 21; (5) L BA 22; (6) L BA 39; (7) L BA 40; (8) L BA 44; (9) L BA 45; (10-18) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas; proportion of patients who showed increases and decreases in (parts of) each ROI in individual fixed effects SPM analyses |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L mid temporal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R superior parietal ↓ L insula ↓ L posterior STG |
Findings notes | These are the regions involved in what the authors interpret as a "general shift" |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 |
Number of control participants | 10 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 54 years, range 33-76 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 14; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 7 years, range 1.4-37.3 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | Sentence-picture matching, lexical decision, phonological similarity, word repetition, sentence repetition, morphological similarity, semantic categorization, sentence acceptability |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L |
Participants notes | 2 of the 14 patients were not stroke, but were post resective surgery |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (there was only one block per condition per run, so condition could be confounded with low frequency drift; also, the length of the sentences is not stated so it is unclear how well the HRF peak aligns with the sparse acquisitions) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 69 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word |
Control condition | Listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Appear similar |
Behavioral data notes | There appears to be a small RT difference (control condition slower) |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | There are more control participants in another paper (Tyler et al., 2010, Cereb Cortex), but the relevant contrast does not seem to be shown in that paper |
Contrast notes | The contrast is intended to identify regions involved in syntactic processing, however it seems possible that there are semantic differences between these conditions also |
First level contrast | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Behavioral data notes | The two groups showed similar differences between RTs in the two conditions of the contrast |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on pp. 3402-3; each group is presented at voxelwise p < .005 (CDT), cluster-corrected p < .05 with GRFT |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L posterior MTG |
Findings notes | Several other potential differences are apparent in the figure, but only the differences tabulated are interpreted in the text |
First level contrast | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | RT difference between early and late targets on grammatical but meaningless sentences (a measure of syntactic processing) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Analyses focuses on RT differences between early and late targets, not on mean RT per se |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activated for the same contrast |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis |
Findings notes | L IFG showed more activation in patients that had a larger target position effect (indicative of better syntactic processing) |
First level contrast | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | RT difference between early and late targets on normal sentences |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activated for the same contrast |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | RT difference between early and late targets on scrambled sentences |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activated for the same contrast |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Damage to L IFG, estimated from T1 signal |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activated for the same contrast |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | No correlation (p = .57) |
First level contrast | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Syntactic processing (presumably the target position effect, though this is not stated) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activated for the same contrast |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | No correlation (p = .41) |
First level contrast | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs listening to scrambled sentences and detecting a target word |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion status of each voxel |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | VBM was used to identify any regions where damage was predictive of activation in the L IFG pars triangularis and orbitalis. Tissue integrity was quantified in terms of T1 signal. Clusterwise correction was used, which is not appropriate for VBM. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Only in the L IFG itself was damage predictive of reduced activation in the L IFG. |
Language | Dutch |
Inclusion criteria | MCA; mRS < 3; able to perform at least 2 out of the 3 tasks |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 13 |
Number of control participants | 13 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 53 ± 14 years, range 29-74 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 4; females: 9) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (right: 13; left: 0; not stated for controls) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 1.3-4.7 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT, BNT, TT |
Aphasia severity | 4 moderate, 4 severe, 3 recovered, 2 mild; all had aphasia initially |
Aphasia type | 5 anomic, 4 Broca's, 3 recovered, 1 Wernicke's |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 6.0-167.3 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | Behavioral data (TT and a naming measure) were also acquired subacutely (mean 26 ± 18 days, range 5-56 days) |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 3036 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | breath holding scan also done to measure hemodynamic responsiveness |
Language condition | Written word-picture matching |
Control condition | Visual decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy not reported for control condition |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Not clearly stated that language tasks were contrasted only with arrow decision task and not rest for the first two contrasts, but this can be inferred |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Visual decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy not reported for control condition |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Not clearly stated that language tasks were contrasted only with arrow decision task and not rest for the first two contrasts, but this can be inferred |
Language condition | Verb generation |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy not reported for control condition |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy not reported for control condition |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Picture-word matching accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Semantic decision accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Overall language measure |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Overall language measure |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Not clear if it was LI for whole language network |
Findings | ↑ LI (language network) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Overall language measure |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Damage to L hemisphere language regions |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Damage to L hemisphere language regions |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Damage to L hemisphere language regions |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R anterior language region (IFG); (2) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Previous (current vs subacute) Δ naming |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Previous (current vs subacute) Δ naming |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Previous (current vs subacute) Δ naming |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Previous (current vs subacute) Δ TT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function; TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Previous (current vs subacute) Δ TT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function; TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ R IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Previous (current vs subacute) Δ TT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (current activation will reflect not just prior recovery, but also current language function; TT not optimal measure of overall language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 7 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior language region (IFG); (2) L posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (3) R anterior language region (IFG); (4) R posterior language region (AG, SMG, STG, MTG); (5) frontal LI; (6) temporal LI; (7) whole network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | WFU pickatlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ R IFG |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 |
Number of control participants | 15 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (reanalysis of same dataset from Tyler et al. (2011)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 56 ± 12 years, range 35-77 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 14; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 8 ± 9 years, range 2-40 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | Sentence-picture matching, grammaticality judgment, lexical decision, phonological discrimination, semantic categorization, sentence repetition, word repetition |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | 1 patient had post-surgical haematoma rather than stroke (per Tyler et al., 2011) |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (length of stimuli not described) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 1059 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (lacks explanation of event durations) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") |
Control condition | Listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Control data in Tyler et al. (2011); L frontal and temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between subordinate and dominant sentences (dominance effect) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ L mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Modulation of L IFG connectivity by dominance effect |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A PPI analysis was carried out with the L IFG as the seed region. Correlations were computed between voxelwise modulation of connectivity with this region, and a behavioral measure of syntactic processing, which was the dominance effect: the difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between subordinate and dominant sentences. The resultant SPM was thresholded at voxelwise p < .01 (CDT), then corrected for multiple corrections based on cluster extent and GRFT using SPM8. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Patients with better syntactic performance had more connectivity from the L IFG seed region to L pMTG and adjacent areas (including the insula); pMTG also significant at voxelwise p < .001 in Figure 2B, corrected for multiple comparisons with GRFT |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Modulation of L pMTG connectivity by dominance effect |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A similar PPI analysis was carried out with the L pMTG as the seed region. Thresholding was the same as in the previous analysis. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 |
Number of control participants | 8 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 40-79 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (males: 5; females: 3; control sex not stated, but reported to be matched) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 48.3 months, range 30-78 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, portions of PALPA, PPT, CLQT |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 74.0-97.8 |
Aphasia type | 6 anomic, 2 recovered |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 23-45 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | No (GE 3 Tesla; model not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (control events took place in the inter-trial interval between language events, and may have been systematically confounded in timing; the total number of functional images acquired is not stated) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (only correct trials are included but it is not stated how incorrect trials were modeled; in general, it is not stated whether the control events were modeled at all) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing scrambled images and saying "pass" |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy/RT not reported for control task |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Reporting is selective, but appears mostly bilateral with slight L-lateralization of language areas |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision (correct trials) |
Control condition | Visual decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy/RT not reported for control task |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Clearly lateralized frontal activation, but very modest temporal activation |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "pass" |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG (oper/tri); (2) L posterior perisylvian (pSTG, pMTG, AG, SMG); (3) R IFG (oper/tri); (4) R posterior perisylvian (pSTG, pMTG, AG, SMG); (5) language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Harvard–Oxford atlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ LI (language network) |
Findings notes | Larger lesions were associated with more R posterior perisylvian activation |
First level contrast | Semantic decision (correct trials) vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG (oper/tri); (2) L posterior perisylvian (pSTG, pMTG, AG, SMG); (3) R IFG (oper/tri); (4) R posterior perisylvian (pSTG, pMTG, AG, SMG); (5) language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Harvard–Oxford atlas |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Moderate aphasia, L MCA |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 (plus 3 excluded: 2 metallic artifact; 1 seizure at time of stroke) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 54.4 ± 12.7 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 4; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 5.3 ± 3.6 years, > 12 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | BNT; phonemic fluency, semantic fluency, complex ideation from BDAE, PPVT, communicative activities log |
Aphasia severity | Moderate |
Aphasia type | 4 Broca's, 3 anomic, 1 anomic/conduction |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~2 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | RTMS to residual activation near Broca's area, 5 sessions/week, 2 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Varian Unity INOVA 4 T) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (timing not clear, because previous studies cited are not all identical in terms of timing) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Tone decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Control data in Kim et al. (2011) and Szaflarski et al. (2008); L frontal and temporal, plus other semantic regions |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Language and control tasks both matched |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | in-house |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | The figure shows a cutoff of z > 10, which would not correspond to p < .05; increases and decreases in Figure 3 do not accord with the data from T1 and T2 in Figure 2, raising concerns about the implementation of the analyses; there is no explicit description of the second level analysis |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R precuneus ↑ R occipital ↑ R basal ganglia ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ R insula ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R posterior STG |
Findings notes | Based on a combination of coordinates in Table 2, and Figure 3 |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Language and control tasks both matched |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI; (3) language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | T1 LI (temporal) is reported to be negative, which does not accord with the voxelwise analysis in Figure 2; increases and decreases in Figure 3 do not accord with the data from T1 and T2 in Figure 2, raising concerns about the implementation of the analyses |
Findings | ↑ LI (language network) ↑ LI (frontal) ↑ LI (temporal) |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 |
Number of control participants | 15 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (not stated, but it seems like most of the patients also participated in Tyler et al. (2010)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 56 years, range 34-77 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 14; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 7 years, > 1.5 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | Sentence-picture matching, grammaticality judgment, lexical decision, phonological discrimination, semantic categorization, sentence repetition, word repetition |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | 1 patient had post-surgical haematoma rather than stroke |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (run length not stated; length of stimuli not described) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated but 1059 per Papoutsi et al. (2011) |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (lacks explanation of event durations) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) |
Control condition | Listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L frontal and parietal; R frontal (but L > R); no L temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") |
Control condition | Listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L frontal and parietal; no L temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") |
Control condition | Listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L frontal, temporal and parietal, R frontal (but L > R) |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") |
Control condition | Listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L frontal and temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 423 |
Findings | ↓ L IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 423 |
Findings | ↓ L IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 423 |
Findings | ↓ L IFG |
Findings notes | Lack of patient activation in pMTG implied in text, but this activation looks fairly similar in patients and controls (c.f. Figure 3C vs 2C) |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences with subordinate resolution ("subordinate") vs listening to ambiguous sentences with dominant resolution ("dominant") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 423 |
Findings | ↓ L IFG ↓ L posterior MTG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Performance on acceptability judgment task (difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between ambiguous and unambiguous sentences) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R insula ↑ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | Also L pMTG but this did not reach significance |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Performance on sentence-picture matching task |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 30 (units not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ R insula ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Performance on word monitoring task |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 10 (units not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ R insula ↑ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between subordinate and dominant sentences (dominance effect) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Plausible fronto-temporo-parietal language regions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Performance on acceptability judgment task (difference in percent of unacceptable judgments between ambiguous and unambiguous sentences) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) IFG pars opercularis; (2) IFG pars triangularis; (3) IFG pars orbitalis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to ambiguous sentences (dominant and subordinate) vs listening to unambiguous sentences ("unambiguous") |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Difference in percentage of unacceptable judgments between subordinate and dominant sentences (dominance effect) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) IFG pars opercularis; (2) IFG pars triangularis; (3) IFG pars orbitalis |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Age 55-85 |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 10 (plus 4 excluded: 3 malfunction of TMS device or claustrophobia; 1 recovered nearly completely prior to intervention) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 59-83 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 5) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 10; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 18-97 days; patients at different subacute stages of recovery) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Type only |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | T1: TT range 0-45 errors; T2: TT range 0-44 errors |
Aphasia type | T1: 5 Wernicke's, 2 Broca's, 2 global, 1 amnestic fluent; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Extent and location |
Lesion extent | Range 0.7-88.9 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—mixed |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/subacute (range 18-97 days post onset); T2: post-treatment, ~2 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Individualized SLT, 45 minutes/day, 5 days/week, 2 weeks; 6 patients underwent rTMS to the R IFG pars triangularis; 4 received vertex (sham) rTMS |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 8 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Control data in Herholz et al. (1996); insufficient to fully validate the contrast |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 (regardless of rTMS) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) IFG LI; (2) superior temporal LI; (3) SMA LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated with rTMS (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) IFG LI; (2) superior temporal LI; (3) SMA LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia with R IFG rTMS (n = 6) T2 vs T1) vs (with sham rTMS (n = 4) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) IFG LI; (2) superior temporal LI; (3) SMA LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | IFG LI was stable in the stimulation group, but shifted to the R in the sham group, yielding a significant difference between groups |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | MCA; moderate-severe aphasia; mRS ≤ 3 |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 16 |
Number of control participants | 32 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes ("part of a larger ongoing study", may overlap with other studies from this group) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 54.4 ± 9.5 years, range 38-78 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 9; females: 7) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 16; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 3.7 ± 3.5 years, range 0.5-11.4 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | TT, PPVT, BNT, semantic and phonemic fluency, complex ideation subtest of BDAE |
Aphasia severity | Moderate-severe; TT mean 25.5 ± 11.3; unclear how to reconcile moderate-severe severity with mostly anomic aphasia |
Aphasia type | Mostly anomic with some non-fluent |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 2.8-248.9 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | No (Phillips 3 Tesla; model not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Mixed |
Total images acquired | 435 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (no description of HRF model, which is important given sparse sampling design) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Verb generation (covert, block) |
Control condition | Finger tapping (block) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Strongly lateralized frontal and temporal activation |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation (overt, event-related) |
Control condition | Noun repetition (event-related) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Somewhat L-lateralized frontal, temporal and parietal activations, but also extensive midline activation |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation (overt, event-related) |
Control condition | Verb generation (covert, event-related) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | N/A |
Control activation notes | Bilateral speech motor activations, but also extensive midline activation |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation (covert, block) vs finger tapping (block) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ LI (temporal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation (overt, event-related) vs noun repetition (event-related) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate and produced less responses on both conditions, but the difference between groups was greater for verb generation |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation (overt, event-related) vs verb generation (covert, event-related) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Overt performance differed, so covert performance probably did too |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | Lack of lateralization in controls makes this analysis difficult to interpret |
First level contrast | Verb generation (overt, event-related) vs noun repetition (event-related) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Overt verb generation accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L MTG; (2) L SFG/CG; (3) left MFG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions activated by the contrast of overt verb generation vs noun repetition in patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation (overt, event-related) vs verb generation (covert, event-related) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Overt verb generation accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R insula/IFG; (2) R STG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Prominent R hemisphere activations for the contrast of overt and covert verb generation in patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Broca's aphasia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 10 (plus 3 excluded: 1 due to a metal implant; 2 for severely non-fluent speech) |
Number of control participants | 20 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 56.9 ± 9.2 years, range 45-75 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (males: 9; females: 4; control sex not matched) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 12; left: 1) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 63.8 ± 64.3 months, range 10-261 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, AoS from ABA |
Aphasia severity | AQ mean 48.5 ± 20.6, range 20.9-73.5 |
Aphasia type | Broca's |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | Demographic data includes excluded patients |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | No (Siemens 3 Tesla; model not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (it appears that each of the three conditions was presented in a separate run) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 180? |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (not described clearly) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences, while producing the same sentences in unison (speech entrainment) |
Control condition | Listening to reversed sentences and viewing a mouth speaking, while producing unrelated sentences |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Behavioral data outside the scanner suggest not matched, but in-scanner behavioral data not reported |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Control and patient data are combined; this contrast activates bilateral anterior insula and posterior MTG, slightly more extensive on the L |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences, while producing the same sentences in unison (speech entrainment) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Rest condition implied but not explicitly described |
Language condition | Listening to reversed sentences and viewing a mouth speaking, while producing unrelated sentences |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Rest condition implied but not explicitly described |
Language condition | Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences and viewing a mouth |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Rest condition implied but not explicitly described |
First level contrast | Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences, while producing the same sentences in unison (speech entrainment) vs listening to reversed sentences and viewing a mouth speaking, while producing unrelated sentences |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Unclear or not stated |
Software | FSL (FEAT 5.98) |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Thresholding not stated |
Findings | ↑ L angular gyrus ↓ L anterior temporal |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 2 |
First level contrast | Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences, while producing the same sentences in unison (speech entrainment) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Unclear or not stated |
Software | FSL (FEAT 5.98) |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Thresholding not stated |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R precuneus ↑ R occipital ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ L supramarginal gyrus |
Findings notes | Some labels changed based on coordinates |
First level contrast | Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences and viewing a mouth vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Unclear or not stated |
Software | FSL (FEAT 5.98) |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Thresholding not stated |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to/watching audiovisual sentences, while producing the same sentences in unison (speech entrainment) vs listening to reversed sentences and viewing a mouth speaking, while producing unrelated sentences |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior insula/IFG pars orbitalis; (2) R anterior insula/IFG pars orbitalis; (3) Broca's area; (4) L MTG; (5) L BA 37; (6) R BA 37 |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions activated in both groups considered together |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | There were no interactions of group by condition; two regions showed main effects of group but this is not pertinent to the contrast |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 29 (plus 1 excluded: contraindications to MRI) |
Number of control participants | 14 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (26 of 30 patients were included in Fridriksson (2010)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 59.2 years, range 33-81 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (males: 14; females: 16; not stated for controls) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 51.1 months, range 6-350 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB |
Aphasia severity | AQ mean 57.9 ± 25.8, range 17.2-95.2 |
Aphasia type | 13 Broca's, 10 anomic, 3 conduction, 2 Wernicke's, 1 global, 1 transcortical motor |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 7.7-420.5 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | Demographic data includes excluded patient |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment/~4 weeks later; note that there were two separate sessions per time point, as well as another two sessions midway through treatment that are not analyzed in this paper |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Anomia treatment using a cueing hierarchy, 3 hours/day, 5 days/week, 2 weeks, with a 1-week gap between the two weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (timing of stimuli within the silent periods is unclear) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 120 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling; 26 patients were also scanned with arterial spin labelling |
Language condition | Picture naming |
Control condition | Viewing abstract pictures |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control data in Fridriksson et al. (2007); motor activations are prominent; there is some L frontal activation but little temporal activation in either hemisphere |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ picture naming accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Change in perilesional non-language regions positively correlated with improvement in accuracy |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ (decrease in) semantic errors |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Change in undamaged non-perilesional language regions negatively correlated with decrease in semantic errors |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ (decrease in) phonological paraphasias |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Change in perilesional language regions, and change in undamaged non-perilesional language regions, negatively correlated with decrease in phonological paraphasias |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1, decrease in) semantic errors |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Change in perilesional language regions correlated with decrease in phonological paraphasias |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1, decrease in) phonological paraphasias |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional L hemisphere language regions; (2) perilesional L hemisphere non-language regions; (3) undamaged non-perilesional L hemisphere language regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on individual lesions and control activation for picture naming |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | Canadian French |
Inclusion criteria | Moderate-severe aphasia; anomia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 9 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 62 ± 6.0 years, range 50-67 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 9; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 110.2 ± 92.5 months, range 50-300 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | Montreal-Toulouse Aphasia Battery, picture naming |
Aphasia severity | Moderate-severe |
Aphasia type | 7 Broca's, 1 Broca's + AoS, 1 Wernicke's + AoS |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 14.6-295.8 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, 3-6 weeks later (after 80% performance on trained items, or 6 weeks) |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Semantic feature analysis, 1 hour/day, 3 days/week, 3-6 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (total images acquired not stated) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (T1: known items; T2: trained items; correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained |
Language condition | Picture naming (known items, correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained |
Language condition | Picture naming (trained items, correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained |
First level contrast | Picture naming (T1: known items; T2: trained items; correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 1780; different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained |
Findings | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↓ L dorsal precentral ↓ L posterior MTG |
Findings notes | Labels based on figures rather than text |
First level contrast | Picture naming (known items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) naming of trained items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 10 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R thalamus |
Findings notes | Labels based on figures and text |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "baba" |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Previous Δ (T2 vs T1) naming of trained items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (T2 activation not an appropriate measure of treatment-induced recovery because it reflects T2 performance) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 10 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Different contrasts at different time points not clearly explained |
Findings | ↑ L somato-motor |
Findings notes | Label based on figure |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | Comprehension deficit |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 20 (plus 1 excluded: excessive head motion) |
Number of control participants | 26 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (patients recruited from database so may have participated in prior studies from this group, but not stated explicitly) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 35.8-90.3 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (males: 16; females: 4; control sex not stated) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 3.5 years, range 0.6-8.6 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | CAT |
Aphasia severity | 11 patients (plus one excluded) had moderate comprehension impairments, 9 had severe comprehension impairments; this distribution was bimodal |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 24.2-403.6 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | Demographic data includes excluded patient |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Sonata 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 488 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (mostly whole brain but convexity or cerebellum excluded in some participants) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to word pairs or reversed word pairs, speaker gender judgment |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Control data in Leff et al. (2008); auditory contrast, not intended to be language contrast |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to word pairs, speaker gender judgment |
Control condition | Listening to reversed word pairs, speaker gender judgment |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Behavioral data not separated by condition |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Control data in Leff et al. (2008); L-lateralized activation of posterior STS |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to word pairs or reversed word pairs, speaker gender judgment vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Moderate aphasia (n = 11) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | Structurally, HG was not significantly damaged in this group |
First level contrast | Listening to word pairs or reversed word pairs, speaker gender judgment vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Severe aphasia (n = 9) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Mixed** (major limitation) |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | MGB: SVC; elsewhere: .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L posterior STG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L thalamus |
Findings notes | Specifically: PT, HG and MGB; structurally, the PT and HG were significantly damaged, but not the MGB |
First level contrast | Listening to word pairs or reversed word pairs, speaker gender judgment vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Severe (n = 9) vs moderate (n = 11) aphasia |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L posterior STG |
Findings notes | Specifically, PT; structurally, severe patients had more damage in HG and PT |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 21 |
Number of control participants | 21 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (unclear how many, if any, patients were included in previous studies from this group; design is identical to Tyler et al. (2010)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 57.4 ± 12.5 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 15; females: 6) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 21; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 6.5 ± 7.5 years, > 1.4 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | Sentence-picture matching |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | 3 of the 21 patients were not stroke, but were post resective surgery |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (there was only one block per condition per run, so condition could be confounded with low frequency drift; also, the length of the sentences is not stated so it is unclear how well the HRF peak aligns with the sparse acquisitions) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 69 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Listening to normal sentences and detecting a target word |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral superior temporal, sensorimotor and visual |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and detecting a target word vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM5 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L mid temporal |
Findings notes | At a more stringent threshold of p < .001, with correction for multiple comparisons based on GRFT and cluster extent, only L HG showed reduced activity in patients |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and detecting a target word vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | See statistical details |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Joint ICA was performed on structural and functional contrast images for each of the two contrasts using FIT 2.0b. Seven components were derived, of which 2 were further investigated since their loadings correlated with relevant behavioral measures. Functional components were thresholded at p < .001, cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons, minimum cluster extent = 1.27 cc. Component 1 was considered a "semantics component" because it correlated with the semantic behavioral measure and not with either of the two syntactic measures. This component did not have any anatomical aspect to it. Component 2 was considered a "syntax component" because it correlated with both syntactic behavioral measures and not with the semantic measure. This conceptualization seems somewhat speculative, given that WPE NP and WPE AP are rather indirect measures of syntactic and semantic processing. Component 2 involved damage to left frontal and insular cortex, and underlying dorsal white matter. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Contrast 1 loaded primarily on the R STG for component 1 (the "semantics component") and on the L ITG for component 2 (the "syntax component"). |
First level contrast | Listening to grammatical but meaningless sentences and detecting a target word vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | See statistical details |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Joint ICA was performed on structural and functional contrast images for each of the two contrasts using FIT 2.0b. Seven components were derived, of which 2 were further investigated since their loadings correlated with relevant behavioral measures. Functional components were thresholded at p < .001, cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons, minimum cluster extent = 1.27 cc. Component 1 was considered a "semantics component" because it correlated with the semantic behavioral measure and not with either of the two syntactic measures. This component did not have any anatomical aspect to it. Component 2 was considered a "syntax component" because it correlated with both syntactic behavioral measures and not with the semantic measure. This conceptualization seems somewhat speculative, given that WPE NP and WPE AP are rather indirect measures of syntactic and semantic processing. Component 2 involved damage to left frontal and insular cortex, and underlying dorsal white matter. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Contrast 2 loaded primarily on the R posterior STG for component 1 (the "semantics component") and on the L posterior STG and L IFG for component 2 (the "syntax component"). |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 27 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (recovered: mean 50 ± 13 years; non-recovered: mean 51 ± 13 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 15; females: 12) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 27; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (recovered: mean 2.1 ± 2.1 years; non-recovered: mean 4.9 ± 3.1 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | TT, BNT, semantic fluency, phonemic fluency, PPVT, complex ideation subtest of BDAE |
Aphasia severity | Recovered: TT mean 43 ± 1, ≥ 41; non-recovered: TT mean 23 ± 12, < 41 |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Recovered: median 9.2 cc, range 2.2-26.5 cc; non-recovered: median 74 cc, range 5.1-206.0 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | No (Phillips 3 Tesla; model not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 330 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Tone decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Accuracy appears similar in the non-recovered group, but not in the recovered group |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Control data in Kim et al. (2011) and Szaflarski et al. (2008); L frontal and temporal, plus other semantic regions |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia not recovered (n = 18) vs recovered (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Interaction of group by condition not reported; non-recovered patients were significantly less accurate only on the semantic decision condition, but they actually showed a smaller difference between conditions than the recovered patients |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 4.16 cc |
Statistical details | Cluster-defining threshold (CDT) p < 0.05 too lenient |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R cerebellum ↓ R posterior STG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) |
Covariate | BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | Circular because defined based on recovered status |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) |
Covariate | Semantic fluency |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | Circular because defined based on recovered status |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) |
Covariate | Single word comprehension (PPVT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | Circular because defined based on recovered status |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) |
Covariate | BDAE complex ideation subtest |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | Circular because defined based on recovered status |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) |
Covariate | Phonemic fluency |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | Circular because defined based on recovered status |
Findings | ↓ R posterior STG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (recovered and non-recovered) |
Covariate | Semantic decision accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral cerebellum; (2) R pSTG; (3) L superior parietal lobule; (4) L superior frontal gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were differentially recruited between recovered and non-recovered patients; average t scores from individual SPMs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | Circular because defined based on recovered status |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 24 (plus 6 excluded: 4 did not tolerate MRI or PET scans; 2 TMS device was defective) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (rTMS group: mean 69.8 ± 8.0 years; sham group: mean 71.2 ± 7.8 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 24; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (rTMS group: mean 37.5 ± 18.5 days; sham group: mean 50.6 ± 22.6 days) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | T1: rTMS group: AAT sum of scores mean 251.5 ± 32.4; sham group: mean 251.1 ± 39.5; T2 not stated |
Aphasia type | T1: rTMS group: 7 Wernicke's, 3 amnestic, 2 global, 1 Broca's; sham group: 5 Wernicke's, 3 Broca's, 2 global, 1 amnestic; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | RTMS group: 233 ± 197 cc; sham group: 244 ± 243 cc; lesion extent in images appears much smaller than the stated volumes |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—mixed |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/subacute (rTMS group: mean 37.5 ± 18.5 days post onset; sham group: mean 50.6 ± 22.6 days post onset); T2 post-treatment, ~2.5 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | RTMS group: inhibitory rTMS over the R IFG pars triangularis + SLT for 45 minutes/day, 5 days/week, 2 weeks; control group: sham TMS + SLT |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 8 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Cites Weiduschat et al. (2011) which in turn cites Herholz et al. (1996) which provides some minimal control data |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia with rTMS (n = 13) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with sham (n = 11) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 2244 |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ R IFG ↓ R posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Findings notes | Approximate interpretation of qualitative patterns shown in Figure 3; T1 R lateralization surprising relative to other findings from this group |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia with rTMS (n = 13) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with sham (n = 11) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Actual LIs are not reported, only change in LI |
Findings | ↑ LI (language network) |
Findings notes | T1 R lateralization surprising relative to other findings from this group |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ AAT total score |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Model did not include treatment group (rTMS vs sham) |
Findings | ↑ LI (language network) |
Findings notes | Patients who improved more showed a greater leftward shift of activation; T1 R lateralization surprising relative to other findings from this group |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Anomia; no severe AoS or dysarthria |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 (plus 9 excluded: 4 for ceiling performance; 5 for technical problems) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (median 48 years, range 35-74 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 10; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 14; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (median 41 months, range 11-72 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Type only |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | 8 Broca's, 3 Wernicke's, 1 fluent non-classifiable, 1 global, 1 transcortical sensory |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA; 2 also had ACA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~6 weeks later (labeled T2 and T3 in paper) |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Lexical therapy, alternating between weeks with phonological and semantic treatment, 4 weeks; 60 out of the 132 items were trained |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (trials too close together (~8 s) and insufficient jitter (1-3 s) for event-related design) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 560 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (all conditions) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | But see control data reported in a subsequent paper (Abel et al., 2015) |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (trained items) |
Control condition | Picture naming (untrained items) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Trained items improved more than untrained items |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (semantic trained items) |
Control condition | Picture naming (phonological trained items) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ R basal ganglia |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ picture naming accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L posterior STS ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ L occipital |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained items) vs picture naming (untrained items) |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Trained items improved more than untrained items |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L thalamus ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R thalamus ↑ R basal ganglia |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (semantic trained items) vs picture naming (phonological trained items) |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differential effects for semantic vs phonological trained items |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R superior parietal ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L occipital ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ R precuneus ↓ R occipital ↓ R anterior cingulate ↓ R posterior cingulate ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with semantic impairment T1 (n = 8) vs with phonological impairment T1 (n = 6) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia with semantic impairment (n = 8) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia with phonological impairment (n = 6) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Phonological patients showed more improvement on trained items |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L occipital ↑ L thalamus ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ R somato-motor ↓ L IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with semantic impairment (n = 8) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L basal ganglia |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (all conditions) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with phonological impairment (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | "at least minimal evidence of non-fluent output"; lesion including precentral gyrus or underlying white matter |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (intention group: mean 72.1 ± 10.5 years; control group: mean 63.0 ± 9.2 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 8; females: 6) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 14; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (intention group: mean 37.4 ± 33.5 months, range 12-87 months; control group: 38.1 ± 37.4 months, range 10-112 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, PPVT |
Aphasia severity | Intention group: AQ mean 65.5 ± 8.3; control group: AQ mean 71.9 ± 11.9 |
Aphasia type | Intention group: 4 conduction, 2 Broca's, 1 anomic; control group: 4 anomic, 1 Broca's, 1 conduction, 1 transcortical motor |
First stroke only? | No |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA, extending frontally at least into the precentral gyrus or underlying white matter |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment; T3: 3 months after the end of treatment |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Word finding therapy for both groups, but the intention group had to produce complex left hand movements, while the control group did not; note that groups were not directly compared in any imaging analyses |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (total images acquired not stated) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | No (not described) |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (not described clearly) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Word generation |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | Contrast not described explicitly but there is only one possible contrast |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with intention treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | Laterality shift for lateral frontal LI, not medial frontal LI |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with intention treatment (n = 6) T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | Laterality shift for both lateral and medial frontal LIs |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with control treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with control treatment (n = 7) T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with intention treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ category-member generation probe performance |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ LI (temporal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with control treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ category-member generation probe performance |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with intention treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ picture naming probe performance |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Word generation vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with control treatment (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ picture naming probe performance |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) lateral frontal LI; (2) medial frontal LI; (3) posterior perisylvian LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | No involvement of ACA territory |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 16 (plus 3 excluded: 2 withdrew after attempting first scan; 1 had severe dysarthria) |
Number of control participants | 17 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 60 years, range 37-84 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 5) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 16; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 4 years, range 6 months-11 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | Not stated |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated, but all had auditory comprehension and repetition deficits, and all could at least attempt to repeat |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L temporal and parietal cortex; 4 extended into the frontal lobe; no lesions involved ACA territory |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | Patients: T1: acclimatization/chronic (but used in some analyses); T2: pre-treatment/chronic (not stated how long after T1); T3: post-treatment/~4 weeks later; controls: T1: pre-training; T2: post-training/~2 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Patients: home-based therapy consisting of auditory discrimination and repetition tasks for 3 or 4 weeks between T2 and T3; control: 2 weeks of similar training using noise vocoded speech |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Intera 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (timing of sentence presentation not described; sparse event-related design, but ITI of only 8 s and consistent linear order of listening and repetition trials could make it difficult to disentangle hemodynamic responses to listening and repeating trials) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 168 (patients); 280 (controls) |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (consistent linear order of listening and repetition trials could make it difficult to disentangle hemodynamic responses to listening and repeating trials) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling; different task structure in controls (two repetition trials per listening trial) raises concerns about comparisons between groups |
Language condition | Listening to sentences |
Control condition | Listening to segmented white noise |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to sentences (patients) or listening to noise vocoded sentences (controls) |
Control condition | Listening to segmented white noise |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences vs listening to segmented white noise |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia (T2 and T3) vs control (T1 and T2) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | Significant difference in accuracy of subsequent repetition |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL (FEAT 5.98) |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L precuneus ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R precuneus ↓ R posterior cingulate |
Findings notes | Findings are approximate since description is partially in terms of networks; at the earlier time point only, patients also showed reduced activity in left ventral prefrontal cortex and right medial planum temporale |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences (patients) or listening to noise vocoded sentences (controls) vs listening to segmented white noise |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia (T2 and T3) vs control (T1 and T2) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | No significant difference in accuracy of subsequent repetition |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL (FEAT 5.98) |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences vs listening to segmented white noise |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia mean of T1, T2, T3 |
Covariate | Picture description score (CAT), mean of T1, T2, T3 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | Referring to accuracy of subsequent repetition; correlation with picture description is not reported |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex/midline superior frontal gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Contrast of listening to vocoded speech and listening to normal speech in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Same result obtained with age and lesion volume included in the model |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R anterior cingulate |
Findings notes | Increased activation of dACC/SFG was correlated with higher scores on picture description |
Language | Italian |
Inclusion criteria | L MCA; comprehension mildly impaired |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 12 |
Number of control participants | 10 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (range 37-79 years; control ages not reported, though reported to be matched) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (males: 7; females: 5; control sex not stated, but reported to be matched) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 12; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (T1: mean 2.2 ± 1.3 days; T2: mean 16.2 ± 1.3 days; T3: mean 190 ± 25.5 days) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT, TT |
Aphasia severity | T1: TT range 2-45; T2: TT range 6-48; T3: TT range 21-48 |
Aphasia type | T1: 8 Broca's, 3 anomic, 1 Wernicke's; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 4.4-158.3 cc (possibly; units stated do not seem correct) |
Lesion location | L MCA; lesions seem very small in Supplementary Figure 1, but are described as more extensive in Supplementary Table 1 |
Participants notes | Treated and untreated groups differed in severity at baseline, albeit not significantly |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—mixed |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment, mean 2.2 ± 1.3 days post onset; T2: post-treatment, mean 16.2 ± 1.3 days post onset; T3: mean 190 ± 25.5 days post onset |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | 6 patients were randomized to receive treatment focusing on verbal comprehension and lexical retrieval for 1 hour/day, 5 days/week between T1 and T2; no patient received treatment after T2 |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Avanto 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (timing of stimuli not clearly described) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 504 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | No (unclear; number of slices not stated) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (model fitting of noise "bip" not clearly described) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment |
Control condition | Listening to reversed speech |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | 10 participants; quite lateralized activity centered on the anterior Sylvian fissure |
Contrast notes | It is mentioned that "noise" was also included on the negative side of the contrast; it is unclear if this refers to the noise "bip", which would be inappropriate |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated T2 (n = 6) vs untreated T2 (n = 6) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (groups were different but not due to treatment) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.9 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 0.16 cc |
Statistical details | Methods report cluster extent threshold (we assume this was done), but figure caption states uncorrected |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R supramarginal gyrus |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 2 |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated T3 (n = 6) vs untreated T3 (n = 6) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (groups were different but not due to treatment) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.9 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 0.16 cc |
Statistical details | Methods report cluster extent threshold (we assume this was done), but figure caption states uncorrected |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L insula ↑ L supramarginal gyrus |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 2; also increases in R IFG and R supramarginal gyrus but only uncorrected |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia treated (n = 6) T2 vs T1) vs (untreated (n = 6) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.9 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 548 |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ R posterior STG ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ R IFG |
Findings notes | Treated patients showed increases in L IFG and R STG, while untreated patients showed increases in L IPL and R IFG |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia treated (n = 6) T3 vs T2) vs (untreated (n = 6) T3 vs T2) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.9 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 548 |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | The two groups were reported to have comparable increases in L hemisphere language areas |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.9 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R posterior STG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.9 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ R insula |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated (n = 6) T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.9 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L insula ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ R insula |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated (n = 6) T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 1.9 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R angular gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia treated (n = 6) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) vs (untreated (n = 6) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG |
Findings notes | Interaction of time by treatment: treated group showed greater L IFG activity at T2 |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ written language (AAT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ naming (AAT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ written language (AAT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences and making a plausibility judgment vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated (n = 6) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ naming (AAT) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L STG; (4) R STG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Based on functional data from patients and controls, but details not stated; a different set of ROIs are mentioned in the results so it is not really clear which set were actually used |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R IFG |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | MCA; mild-moderate non-fluent aphasia; no severe comprehension deficit |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 6 (plus 6 excluded: 4 for health risks; 2 for technical problems and data loss) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 41-76 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 6; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 17-234 months (including excluded patients)) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | BDAE, TT |
Aphasia severity | Mild-moderate; T1: TT range 15-49 errors (including 2 excluded patients) |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | Patient numbers in tables 1 and 2 appear not to correspond with patient numbers later in the paper |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~2 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | CIAT, 3-4 hours/day, 5 days/week, 2 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 76 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Listening to sentences (high and low ambiguity) |
Control condition | Listening to signal-correlated noise |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Some control data in Rodd et al. (2005), but half of the participants were performing a probe judgment task, unlike in the present study |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to high ambiguity sentences |
Control condition | Listening to low ambiguity sentences |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Some control data in Rodd et al. (2005), but half of the participants were performing a probe judgment task, unlike in the present study |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to sentences (high and low ambiguity) vs listening to signal-correlated noise |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative generalization across individuals on pp. 8-9 |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to high ambiguity sentences vs listening to low ambiguity sentences |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) L ITG; (4) R ITG; the temporal ROIs are described as STG but they seem to be in the ITG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Defined based on control data from Rodd et al. (2005) but the coordinates do not match so it is not clear exactly how they were defined |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | ANOVA of timepoint by hemisphere by site, with a significant interaction of timepoint by hemisphere |
Findings | ↑ R IFG ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | All signal changes were negative (i.e. less activation for ambiguous sentences), making interpretation challenging |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | Wernicke's aphasia (impaired spoken single word comprehension, impaired single word repetition, fluent, sentence-like speech with phonological/neologistic errors) |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 12 |
Number of control participants | 12 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 70.1 ± 8.7 years, range 59-87 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 10; females: 2) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 12; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 7-84 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | BDAE, PPT, word-to-picture matching test from Cambridge Semantic Battery, single word reading aloud from PALPA |
Aphasia severity | BDAE comprehension range 6-26 (out of 32); BDAE comprehension scores and percentiles do not seem entirely commensurate |
Aphasia type | All Wernicke's |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA; all involved STG extending into IPL and temporoparietal junction; 8 extending into MTL; 4 extending into inferior frontal |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (each condition was acquired in a separate run, which is suboptimal) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 417 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | spin echo fMRI to minimize ATL dropout |
Language condition | Semantic decision (written word and picture) |
Control condition | Visual decision and rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | Not comparable because the control condition includes rest |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Control data are provided in Table 6 for contrasts of written word semantic decision vs dual baseline, and picture semantic decision vs dual baseline, but not for the main effect of semantic decision; these data suggest that the contrast activates ventral temporal regions bilaterally |
Contrast notes | Two contrasts are described: (1) written word judgment versus a dual baseline of visual judgment and rest; (2) picture judgment versus a dual baseline of visual judgment and rest; these two primary contrasts are reported in patients and controls separately, but no between-group contrasts are reported, so these contrasts are excluded from our review; rather, the between-groups analyses in the paper take the form of ANOVAs; the main effect of group in these ANOVAs collapses across the two described contrasts, therefore we have coded the contrast as the average of the two described contrasts; the exact nature of the computation of dual baseline contrasts is not described |
First level contrast | Semantic decision (written word and picture) vs visual decision and rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Patients also less accurate on control condition, but control condition includes rest so coded based on language condition only |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 4 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Dual baseline computation not explained |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L cerebellum ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R mid temporal ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ R posterior cingulate |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision (written word and picture) vs visual decision and rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Patients also less accurate on control condition, but control condition includes rest so coded based on language condition only |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 10 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L anterior fusiform gyrus; (2) L temporal pole; (3) L anterior STS; (4) L IFG; (5) L ventral occipito-temporal; (6-10) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around functional peaks from literature |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Dual baseline computation not explained |
Findings | ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 32 |
Number of control participants | 32 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (some participants included in Allendorfer et al. (2012)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 51.8 ± 15.1 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 18; females: 14) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 3.2 ± 3.1 years, > 6 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | Not stated |
Aphasia severity | "complete or almost complete" recovery in a "substantial proportion" of the patients |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | 60.1 ± 57.5 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | One participant was < 18 years old at time of stroke; there was also a perinatal stroke group, not relevant for this review; 3 participants were excluded but it is not stated whether they were adult or perinatal patients. |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla, except for 1 patient and 1 control on a Bruker 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 165 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation |
Control condition | Finger tapping |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control data in Szaflarski et al. (2008); frontal activation L-lateralized, temporal less so |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | CCHIPS |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on pp. 5-6 (page numbers refer to PMC author manuscript) |
Findings | ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L occipital ↓ R occipital |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporal LI; (3) language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | Temporal LI was also marginally significantly reduced (p = .08) |
Language | Australian English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 |
Number of control participants | 14 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 56.4 + 9.2 years; range 41-69 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 3; females: 5) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 52.3 + 49.8 months; range 17-170 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, PPT, CAT, picture naming from International Picture Naming Project Database |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 57.3-91.6; 5 mild, 2 moderate, 1 mild-moderate |
Aphasia type | 6 anomic, 2 conduction |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L hemisphere |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, 5-6 weeks later; note that "immediate improvement" was measured at the end of SLT, a week or two prior to T2 scan |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | SLT with alternating semantic and phonological sessions, 3 days/week, 4 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Bruker MedSpec 4 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 610 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | slow event-related design; sparse sampling |
Language condition | Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing scrambled images |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Control data are described for naming untrained items; the data are reported only briefly in the text; it is notable that no speech motor, visual, or auditory activations are reported, as might be expected in a picture naming task |
Contrast notes | Correct and incorrect trials were apparently modeled separately, but this is not clearly stated, nor are the criteria for deciding whether trials were correct; it is generally not clear which contrasts exactly were run |
Language condition | Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) |
Control condition | Viewing scrambled images |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Control data are described for naming untrained items; the data are reported only briefly in the text; it is notable that no speech motor, visual, or auditory activations are reported, as might be expected in a picture naming task |
Contrast notes | Correct and incorrect trials were apparently modeled separately, but this is not clearly stated, nor are the criteria for deciding whether trials were correct; it is generally not clear which contrasts exactly were run |
First level contrast | Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming (phonological treated items) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 0.999 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming (semantic treated items) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 0.999 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L basal ganglia |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Previous Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming (phonological treated items) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (T2 activation not an appropriate measure of treatment-induced recovery because it reflects T2 performance) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 0.999 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ R precuneus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Previous Δ (T2 vs T1) picture naming (semantic treated items) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (T2 activation not an appropriate measure of treatment-induced recovery because it reflects T2 performance) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 0.999 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent outcome (T2) picture naming |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (not appropriate to correlate T1 imaging with T2 behavior without T1 behavior in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 0.999 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent outcome (T2) picture naming |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (not appropriate to correlate T1 imaging with T2 behavior without T1 behavior in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 0.999 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (phonological trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Picture naming T2 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 0.999 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (semantic trained items, correct trials) vs viewing scrambled images |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Picture naming T2 |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .005 |
Cluster extent | 0.999 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Anomia; no severe AoS or dysarthria |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 (plus 9 excluded: 4 for ceiling performance; 5 for technical problems) |
Number of control participants | 14 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (same dataset as Abel et al. (2014)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (median 48 years, range 35-74 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 10; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 14; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (median 41 months, range 11-72 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Type only |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | 8 Broca's, 3 Wernicke's, 1 fluent non-classifiable, 1 global, 1 transcortical sensory |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA; 2 also had ACA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~6 weeks later (labeled T2 and T3 in paper) |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Lexical therapy, alternating between weeks with phonological and semantic treatment, 4 weeks; 60 out of the 132 items were trained |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (trials too close together (~8 s) and insufficient jitter (1-3 s) for event-related design) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 560 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral somato-motor, auditory and to a lesser extent higher level visual regions; finite impulse analysis only |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | RT shorter at T2 |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L dorsal precentral ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ L precuneus ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R somato-motor ↓ R precuneus ↓ R posterior STS ↓ R posterior MTG ↓ R posterior cingulate ↓ R cerebellum ↓ R thalamus ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Controls responded more quickly |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R precuneus ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L Heschl's gyrus ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L thalamus ↓ L basal ganglia ↓ R insula ↓ R somato-motor ↓ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | (Aphasia T2 vs T1) vs (control T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | RT not reported for controls |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 11 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L precuneus ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L basal ganglia ↓ R precuneus ↓ R posterior STS ↓ R posterior MTG ↓ R posterior cingulate ↓ R thalamus ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | RT not reported for controls |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison between activation in the first 5 TRs after each stimulus on p. 1101 |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | The time course of response is stated to be similar in patients and controls, however the response in patients appears like it could be a couple of seconds slower |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | RT not reported for controls |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Joint ICA was performed on structural and functional contrast images using FIT 1.2c. Three of the 7 components differed between groups in their loadings. Components were thresholded at z > 3.09, not corrected for multiple comparisons. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Three structural-functional components are described in Figure 5 and Table 4. Functional activations are generally small and do not obviously relate to language processing. It is mentioned in the supplementary results that "the lesion maps may dominate estimation of the mixing parameter" (p. 10). |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Impaired naming |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 |
Number of control participants | 8 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 58 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 7; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 15-157 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, PPT, CLQT |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 48.0-97.2 |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | 24.2-431.6 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA except for one patient with R MCA and aphasia |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~10 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Semantic feature-based treatment, 10 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (picture and scrambled conditions have different durations; ITI 2-4 s seems too short; total images acquired not stated) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | controls were run on two different sets of parameters, neither of which was the same as the patients |
Language condition | Picture naming (trained) |
Control condition | Viewing scrambled images and saying "skip" |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Overlap of individual participant activation maps; somewhat lateralized frontal and temporal, but also bilateral occipito-temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic feature decision |
Control condition | Visual decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Overlap of individual participant activation maps; somewhat lateralized frontal and temporal, but also bilateral occipito-temporal |
Contrast notes | This contrast inferred but not described |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained) vs viewing scrambled images and saying "skip" |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Analyses were carried out in individual patients at p < .001, uncorrected; regions were considered activated when they were found in 6 or more (out of 8) patients |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R posterior MTG ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | Regions are approximate since only broad regions are described in Table 6 |
First level contrast | Semantic feature decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Analyses were carried out in individual patients at p < .001, uncorrected; regions were considered activated when they were found in 6 or more (out of 8) patients |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R posterior MTG |
Findings notes | Regions are approximate since only broad regions are described in Table 7 |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 10 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 59 years, range 47-75 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 7; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 10; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 7-134 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, subtests from PALPA, PPT, CLQT |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 41.7-99.2 |
Aphasia type | 6 anomic, 2 conduction, 1 Broca's, 1 transcortical motor |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 0.3-256.0 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, up to 10 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Semantic feature-based treatment, 2 hours/day, 2 days/week, up to 10 weeks (depending on when criterion reached) |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (total images acquired not stated; ITI of 1-3 s seems short) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Concreteness judgment (abstract words, correct trials) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | The concreteness judgment task was compared to the letter string judgment task to define ROIs for connectivity analysis, but the group analysis meeting criteria for this review appears to be based only on comparisons between time points on the concreteness judgment conditions |
Language condition | Concreteness judgment (concrete words, correct trials) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | The concreteness judgment task was compared to the letter string judgment task to define ROIs for connectivity analysis, but the group analysis meeting criteria for this review appears to be based only on comparisons between time points on the concreteness judgment conditions |
First level contrast | Concreteness judgment (abstract words, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with response to treatment (n = 9) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | Images show peaks instead of activations |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Concreteness judgment (concrete words, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with generalization of treatment effects to concrete words (n = 7) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | Images show peaks instead of activations |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L inferior parietal lobule ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L occipital ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R posterior cingulate |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | No severe receptive aphasia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 53 |
Number of control participants | 24 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 62 ± 14 years, range 26-83 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (males: 32; females: 21; controls were mostly female, unlike patients) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 50; left: 3) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 111 ± 27 days, range 84-200 days) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | CAT, QPA |
Aphasia severity | "relatively mild stroke"; 17 patients were so mild that they were not aphasic per the CAT |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | No |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Mean 25.4 ± 13.5 cc, range 0.3-168.0 cc |
Lesion location | L; modest R involvement in 7 cases |
Participants notes | Prior strokes were allowed only if no aphasia resulted |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 213 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling; mini-blocks of 2-4 trials |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Control data for univariate analysis in Geranmayeh et al. (2014), but note that the present paper does not describe a univariate analysis; control activations reflect speech rather than language |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Counting |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Control data for univariate analysis in Geranmayeh et al. (2014), but note that the present paper does not describe a univariate analysis; control activations are L frontal, L pSTS, L SMA, L > R occipito-temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Target decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Identified using ICA in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs counting |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Identified using ICA in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L IFG ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs target decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Identified using ICA in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Activity was compared between pairs of ICA-derived networks. However, circularity was introduced because the networks were defined based on the control group. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Patients showed greater differential activation than controls between (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (3) cingulo-opercular network and the DMN. |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Multiple regression was used to determine whether differential activation between networks was predictive of the behavioral measure: appropriate information-carrying words. There is no issue of circularity with this analysis since it involved only individuals with aphasia. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Differential activation between L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN was positively correlated with AICW. Differential activation between R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN was negatively correlated with AICW. |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | PPI analyses were used to investigate how the speech condition modulated functional connectivity between (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN. However, circularity was introduced because the networks were defined based on the control group. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | In controls, the L FTP network reduced connectivity with the DMN during speech, while the R FTP network increased connectivity with the DMN during speech. Both of these interactions were significantly decreased in patients. This was also true for contrasts 2 and 3. |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Moderate aphasia, L MCA |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 (plus 3 excluded: 2 metallic artifact; 1 seizure at time of stroke) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (same patients as Szaflarski et al. (2011); different fMRI paradigm acquired in the same sessions) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 54.4 ± 12.7 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 4; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 5.3 ± 3.6 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | BNT; phonemic fluency, semantic fluency, complex ideation from BDAE, PPVT, communicative activities log |
Aphasia severity | Moderate |
Aphasia type | 4 Broca's, 3 anomic, 1 anomic/conduction |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 1.4-52.5 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~2 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | RTMS to residual activation near Broca's area, 5 sessions/week, 2 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Varian Unity INOVA 4 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 140 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation |
Control condition | Finger tapping |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control data in Szaflarski et al. (2008); frontal activation L-lateralized, temporal less so |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | None |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R thalamus ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Based on description in text; it is noted that no regions survived FDR correction |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) frontal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | First principal component of 8 mm spheres defined based on previously reported control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Lesion volume included in model |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↓ R IFG ↑ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ semantic fluency |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG; (2) R IFG; (3) frontal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | First principal component of 8 mm spheres defined based on previously reported control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Lesion volume included in model |
Findings | ↓ R IFG |
Findings notes | Decreased R IFG activation was correlated with improved semantic fluency |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | PPI analyses were used to investigate change over time in modulation by verb generation of functional connectivity between L IFG and R IFG. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant decrease in modulation by verb generation of functional connectivity between L IFG and R IFG (p = 0.03). Prior to TMS, connectivity increased during verb generation compared to finger tapping, while after TMS, connectivity decreased during verb generation compared to finger tapping. |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ semantic fluency in association with modulation of interhemispheric IFG connectivity by verb generation |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | PPI analyses were used to investigate whether change over time in modulation by verb generation of functional connectivity between L IFG and R IFG was associated with changes in semantic fluency scores, which are limited as a measure of language improvement. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (patients improved only on semantic fluency) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | PPI analyses were used to investigate change over time in modulation by verb generation of functional connectivity between R IFG and all other brain regions. Voxelwise p < .001, not corrected for multiple comparisons. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Reduced connectivity was observed in the L IFG pars opercularis, L anterior temporal lobe, L occipital lobe, L basal ganglia, R SMA and pre-SMA, R somato-motor cortex, R posterior MTG, and R cerebellum. It is noted that no regions survived FDR correction. |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Some spared tissue in L IFG |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 (plus 2 excluded: 1 had no spared tissue in the L IFG; 1 had a R hemisphere stroke) |
Number of control participants | 8 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (although not stated, it is apparent that many of the patients were included in Sandberg et al. (2015)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 59.7 years, range 48-75 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 10; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 14; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 6 years, range 6 months-13 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, PPT, CLQT |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 48.0-99.2 |
Aphasia type | 4 anomic, 2 Broca's, 2 conduction, 2 transcortical motor, 1 anomic or transcortical motor, 1 Broca's or conduction, 1 "N/A", 1 Wernicke's or conduction |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (total images acquired not stated) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | no smoothing |
Language condition | Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) |
Control condition | Visual decision or pseudoword identity decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | 8 patients and 4 controls performed one paradigm, while 6 patients and 4 controls performed another; the data were combined based on the assumption that similar processes were implicated by the two contrasts |
First level contrast | Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Semantic feature decision accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 16 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars opercularis; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4) L SFG; (5) L MFG; (6) L MTG; (7) L AG/SMG; (8) L ACC; (9-16) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | WAB AQ |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 16 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars opercularis; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4) L SFG; (5) L MFG; (6) L MTG; (7) L AG/SMG; (8) L ACC; (9-16) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 16 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars opercularis; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4) L SFG; (5) L MFG; (6) L MTG; (7) L AG/SMG; (8) L ACC; (9-16) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | PPT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 16 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG pars orbitalis; (2) L IFG pars opercularis; (3) L IFG pars triangularis; (4) L SFG; (5) L MFG; (6) L MTG; (7) L AG/SMG; (8) L ACC; (9-16) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No correlation between lesion volume and accuracy, not clear whether control condition accuracy was also tested |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 8 |
What are the ROI(s)? | As above but only in the R hemisphere |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior MTG |
Findings notes | MTG included anterior too; SMG/AG was single ROI |
First level contrast | Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion status of 8 ROIs |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Multivariate mixed-effects linear regression analyses were used to identify relationships between structural damage to 8 regions, and functional activation in 16 regions. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons based on FDR. This analysis was not described in sufficient detail. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Sparing of the L ACC and L SFG was associated with more functional activation in many regions, however this is difficult to interpret since these regions were largely or completely spared in many patients. Damage to the L IFG pars orbitalis, L MTG and L AG/SMG was associated with activation of the L ACC, L SFG (and other regions) potentially indicative of compensatory processing. |
First level contrast | Semantic feature decision (6 patients, 4 controls) or semantic relatedness decision (8 patients, 4 controls) vs visual decision or pseudoword identity decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations were computed between functional activation in 16 regions, and qualitatively compared between patients and controls (p. 123). There was no correction for multiple comparisons. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | In controls, all regions were generally correlated with one another. This was largely true in patients too, with the exception of the R IFG pars orbitalis, which was negatively correlated with the L IFG. |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | L hand motor area spared; mild aphasia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 16 |
Number of control participants | 16 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 56.7 ± 10.1 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 10; females: 6) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 16; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 54.3 ± 45.3 months, range 12-169 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | Mild |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 9.7-165.1 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA not including hand motor area |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1/T2: chronic; tDCS and sham sessions in randomized order |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 100 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Picture naming |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia after tDCS (n = 16) vs aphasia after sham stimulation (n = 16); same patients, order counterbalanced, repeated measures |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no behavioral difference) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and stringent voxelwise p |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | Repeated measures |
Findings | ↓ L insula ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ R occipital ↓ R anterior cingulate |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia after sham stimulation (n = 16) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients named > 90% correctly in all sessions; control RT not reported |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral anterior cingulate; (2) L insula; (3) R lingual gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were less active in patients with tDCS vs sham |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R anterior cingulate |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia after tDCS (n = 16) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients named > 90% correctly in all sessions; control RT not reported |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral anterior cingulate; (2) L insula; (3) R lingual gyrus |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were less active in patients with tDCS vs sham |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia after tDCS (n = 16) vs aphasia after sham stimulation (n = 16); same patients, order counterbalanced, repeated measures |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no behavioral difference) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | ICA was used to derive three task-relevant components: language, motor and visual. Thresholding of the functional maps is not described, but they appear to reflect coherent components of a picture naming network. These components were compared between stimulation conditions in terms of mean activity and power in three frequency bins. It should be noted that the language component is left-lateralized, unlike the model-based picture naming contrast. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Activity in the language component was greater in the tDCS condition. In the frequency domain, the tDCS condition showed reduced power in the highest frequency bin, and increased power in the lowest frequency bin. |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia after sham stimulation (n = 16) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | ICA was used to derive three task-relevant components: language, motor and visual. Thresholding of the functional maps is not described, but they appear to reflect coherent components of a picture naming network. These components were compared between stimulation conditions in terms of mean activity and power in three frequency bins. It should be noted that the language component is left-lateralized, unlike the model-based picture naming contrast. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Mean activity of these components did not differ between patients and controls. However, patients showed increased power in the middle frequency bin of the visual component. |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia after tDCS (n = 16) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | ICA was used to derive three task-relevant components: language, motor and visual. Thresholding of the functional maps is not described, but they appear to reflect coherent components of a picture naming network. These components were compared between stimulation conditions in terms of mean activity and power in three frequency bins. It should be noted that the language component is left-lateralized, unlike the model-based picture naming contrast. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 27 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (patients are a subset of those in Geranmayeh et al. (2016)) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 59.1 ± 10.8 years, range 39-77 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 18; females: 9) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 26; left: 1) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (T1: 15 ± 7.6 days (range 5-35 days); T2: 108 ± 26 days (range 87-200 days)) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | CAT, QPA |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | No |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Mean 41.4 ± 44.4 cc, range 3.8-173.9 cc |
Lesion location | L; modest R involvement in 3 cases |
Participants notes | 24 control participants are described, but no imaging data from the controls are analyzed in this paper |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: 15 ± 7.6 days (range 5-35 days); T2: 108 ± 26 days (range 87-200 days) |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Variable modest amounts of SLT (range 0-18 hours) reported in Supplementary Table 1 |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 213 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling; mini-blocks of 2-4 trials |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Control data in Geranmayeh et al. (2014); speech not language; relevant activations are bilateral |
Contrast notes | Not entirely clear that the whole brain analysis is indeed propositional speech production vs rest; a contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting is also used to define the preSMA/dACC ROI |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia mean of T1, T2 |
Covariate | Simultaneous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (potentially confounded by T1 and T2 language function; language function at T1 was predictive of change in language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | FSL |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 1.6 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior STS ↑ R anterior cingulate |
Findings notes | Findings based on figures and coordinates; the pre-SMA/dACC peak noted to survive FWE correction at p < .001 |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Number of AICW increased |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L pre-SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | No main effect of session in session by language recovery ANOVA |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ number of appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L pre-SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | No interaction of session by language recovery in ANOVA |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia mean of T1, T2 |
Covariate | Simultaneous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (potentially confounded by T1 and T2 language function; language function at T1 was predictive of change in language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L pre-SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | Patients with more pre-SMA activity improved more |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia mean of T1, T2 |
Covariate | Simultaneous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (potentially confounded by T1 and T2 language function; language function at T1 was predictive of change in language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L pre-SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion size covariate |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | Patients with more pre-SMA activity improved more |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia mean of T1, T2 |
Covariate | Simultaneous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes (this analysis is appropriate because T1 behavior is included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L pre-SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion size, T1 performance, and age covariates |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | Patients with more pre-SMA activity improved more |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia mean of T1, T2 |
Covariate | Subsequent outcome (T2) number of appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (mathematically equivalent to the previous analysis, because of the inclusion of T1 performance as a covariate) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L pre-SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion size, T1 performance, and age covariates |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (potentially confounded by T1 language function; language function at T1 was predictive of change in language function) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L pre-SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Previous Δ (T2 vs T1) number of appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (the logic behind correlating activation changes and language outcome is unclear) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | T1 AICW correlated with change in AICW, but not stated whether T2 AICW correlated with change in AICW |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L pre-SMA |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak voxel of the contrast of target decision vs mean of propositional speech and counting in people with aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 43 |
Number of control participants | 43 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (same dataset as Griffis et al. (2017) Hum Brain Mapp) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 53 ± 15 years, range 23-90 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 25; females: 18) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 41; left: 2) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 1-14 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | BNT, semantic fluency, phonemic fluency |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Mean 105.2 ± 76.3 cc |
Lesion location | L |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | No (Siemens Allegra 3 Tesla or Philips 3 Tesla; model not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 165 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Tone decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Tone decision accuracy not reported |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Temporal activation is mid MTG and AG rather than pSTS |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Semantic decision accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L AG and bilateral midline components of the canonical semantic network, along with reduced activity in R frontal, temporal and parietal regions; (2) bilateral IFG pars orbitalis; (3) L IFG and DLPFC along with bilateral midline regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs are mixing coefficients of functional networks arising from mCCA + jICA that were differently represented in the patient and control groups |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R posterior cingulate ↓ L insula ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsal precentral ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | All 3 networks were significantly correlated; analysis of networks so involvement of each individual region cannot be assured |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Average of semantic and phonemic fluency |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L AG and bilateral midline components of the canonical semantic network, along with reduced activity in R frontal, temporal and parietal regions; (2) bilateral IFG pars orbitalis; (3) L IFG and DLPFC along with bilateral midline regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs are mixing coefficients of functional networks arising from mCCA + jICA that were differently represented in the patient and control groups |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R posterior cingulate ↓ L insula ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsal precentral ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | Networks 1 and 3 were significantly correlated; analysis of networks so involvement of each individual region cannot be assured |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L AG and bilateral midline components of the canonical semantic network, along with reduced activity in R frontal, temporal and parietal regions; (2) bilateral IFG pars orbitalis; (3) L IFG and DLPFC along with bilateral midline regions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | ROIs are mixing coefficients of functional networks arising from mCCA + jICA that were differently represented in the patient and control groups |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R posterior cingulate ↓ L insula ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsal precentral ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R posterior STG ↓ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | Networks 1 and 3 were significantly correlated; analysis of networks so involvement of each individual region cannot be assured |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Multimodal canonical correlation analysis (mCCA) and joint ICA were used to identify 3 joint ICs (structural/functional) that were differently represented in the patient and control groups. Although there was no correction for multiple comparisons when the functional maps were thresholded, the maps for the three networks each appeared to relate to coherent parts of the semantic network. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | The first joint IC comprised preservation of tissue in L posterior temporo-parietal region, activity in the L AG and bilateral midline components of the canonical semantic network, and reduced activity in R frontal, temporal and parietal regions. The second joint IC comprised preservation of tissue in the the L basal ganglia/insula region, and activity predominantly in the IFG pars orbitalis bilaterally. The third joint IC comprised preservation of the L IFG and activity in the L IFG and DLPFC along with bilateral midline regions. The first joint IC was considered to provide more robust evidence for structure-function relationships than the other two, because it was the only one where individual structural and functional mixing coefficients remained correlated even when lesion volume was included as a covariate. |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 43 |
Number of control participants | 43 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (data were collected as part of "several separate studies") |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 53 ± 15 years, range 23-90 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 25; females: 18) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 41; left: 2) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 1-14 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | BNT, semantic fluency, phonemic fluency |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Mean 105.2 ± 76.3 cc |
Lesion location | L |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | No (Siemens Allegra 3 Tesla or Philips 3 Tesla; model not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 165 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Tone decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Tone decision accuracy not reported |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Temporal activation is mid MTG and AG rather than pSTS |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Semantic decision accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12/in-house |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 126 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ L brainstem ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital ↑ R brainstem ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ L somato-motor |
Findings notes | Based on figure and table; larger activations are compelling; smaller activations are not due to lenient correction approach |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Average of semantic and phonemic fluency |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12/in-house |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 126 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STS ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L brainstem ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R occipital ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R hippocampus/MTL ↓ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | Based on figure and table; larger activations are compelling; smaller activations are not due to lenient correction approach |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12/in-house |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 126 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Based on figure and table; larger activations are compelling; smaller activations are not due to lenient correction approach |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | R hemisphere |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12/in-house |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 126 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R orbitofrontal ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R cerebellum ↓ R thalamus |
Findings notes | Based on figure and table; larger activations are compelling; smaller activations are not due to lenient correction approach |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) overall canonical semantic network (CSN); (2) L CSN; (3) R CSN; (4) mirror L CSN in R; (5) out-of-network CSN in R |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control data |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | Circular because ROI defined in one group |
Findings | ↓ L IFG ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L angular gyrus ↓ L precuneus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L occipital ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R IFG ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R angular gyrus ↓ R precuneus ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R occipital ↓ R posterior cingulate ↓ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Results are for whole networks of regions, so individual regions cannot be assured; out-of-network R regions not listed since they were not significant in ROI 5 (only in ROI 4) |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) overall canonical semantic network (CSN); (2) L CSN; (3) R CSN; (4) mirror L CSN in R; (5) out-of-network CSN in R |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control data |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Semantic decision accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | CSN |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control data |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Correlation calculated for the whole network of regions, so correlation of individual regions cannot be assured |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Average of semantic and phonemic fluency |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | CSN |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control data |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Correlation calculated for the whole network of regions, so correlation of individual regions cannot be assured |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | CSN |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control data |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↑ L IFG ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | Correlation calculated for the whole network of regions, so correlation of individual regions cannot be assured |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activation magnitudes in the L and R canonical semantic network (CSN) were compared between groups. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activation magnitudes in the L CSN and R mirrored CSN were compared between groups. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Correlations between activations in the L CSN and the mirrored L CSN in the R hemisphere were stronger in patients than controls. |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activation magnitudes in the L CSN and R out-of-network homotopic regions were compared between groups. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Correlations between activations in the L CSN and R out-of-network homotopic regions were stronger in patients than controls. |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | The difference in activation between the L CSN and R CSN was compared between patients and controls. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | The difference in activation between the L CSN and mirror L CSN in the R was compared between patients and controls. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | The difference was smaller in patients. |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Semantic decision accuracy not matched, but tone decision accuracy not reported |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | The difference in activation between the R CSN and out-of-network homotopic regions in the R was compared between patients and controls. However, this analysis is circular because the CSN ROIs were defined based on controls only. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | The difference was smaller in patients. |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Interactions of semantic fluency and naming measures by lesion size |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | For the 4 R hemisphere regions that were more activated in patients with larger lesions (SPM analysis 4), analyses were carried out to determine whether the semantic fluency or naming measures were differentially impacted by activation depending on whether lesions were larger or smaller. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | For 1 of the 4 regions (R SMA), there were significant interactions such that in patients with larger lesions, more activation was associated with higher semantic fluency scores and higher BNT scores, while in patients with smaller lesions, more activation was associated with lower fluency and BNT scores. There was a similar relationship with semantic fluency in the R IFG pars opercularis but only at p(FDR) = 0.07. |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Mild-moderate non-fluent aphasia; relatively intact comprehension; able to produce meaningful words and phrases |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 6 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 47-75 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 6; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 6-102 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | BDAE, BNT |
Aphasia severity | Mild-moderate |
Aphasia type | All non-fluent |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Range 36.6-252.1 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, 2 months after treatment; T3: 6 months after treatment (the 2-month time point was not included in analysis because there was no significant behavioral effect at that time) |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Inhibitory rTMS to R IFG, 10 days |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 200 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming |
Control condition | Viewing patterns |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing patterns |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on pp. 138-9 |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L occipital ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R occipital ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Findings notes | Based on Figure 5 and Table 4 |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | Anomia; good single word comprehension; relatively spared word and nonword repetition; no AoS; spared or partially spared L IFG |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 18 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 50 ± 12 years, range 21-67 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 12; females: 6) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 18; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 61 ± 58 months, range 5-264 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | BNT, one CAT subtest, two PALPA subtests |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~6 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Anomia treatment (computer-based practice), 2+ hours/day, 6 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 696 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Condition | Response type | Repetitions | All groups could do? | All individuals could do? |
picture naming (untrained items, word cue) | Word (overt) | 54 | Yes | Unknown |
picture naming (untrained items, initial phonemes cue) | Word (overt) | 54 | Yes | Unknown |
picture naming (untrained items, final phonemes cue) | Word (overt) | 54 | Yes | Unknown |
picture naming (untrained items, no cue) | Word (overt) | 54 | Yes | Unknown |
picture naming (trained items, word cue) | Word (overt) | 53 | Yes | Unknown |
picture naming (trained items, initial phonemes cue) | Word (overt) | 53 | Yes | Unknown |
picture naming (trained items, final phonemes cue) | Word (overt) | 53 | Yes | Unknown |
picture naming (trained items, no cue) | Word (overt) | 53 | Yes | Unknown |
rest | None | implicit baseline | N/A | N/A |
Language condition | Picture naming (all conditions, correct trials) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | It is difficult to determine exactly what contrasts were employed |
Language condition | Picture naming (untrained items, no cue, correct trials) |
Control condition | Picture naming (trained items, no cue, correct trials) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Untrained items significantly slower at T2 |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | It is difficult to determine exactly what contrasts were employed |
First level contrast | Picture naming (untrained items, no cue, correct trials) vs picture naming (trained items, no cue, correct trials) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | "a change in un-cued naming RT" (exact measure unclear) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (unclear whether behavioral measure is longitudinal) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R anterior insula; (2) R IFG; (3) dorsal anterior cingulate; (4) L premotor cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peaks (only with SVC) for the main effect of untrained (4 conditions) vs trained (4 conditions) in T2 aphasia |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Unclear what the behavioral measure was exactly |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R insula |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | At least mild aphasia per TT |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 19 |
Number of control participants | 38 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (patients are a subset of the 24 participants in Szaflarski et al. (2015), a clinical trial on CIAT) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (CIAT group: mean 58.0 ± 10.6 years; untreated group: mean 50.3 ± 13.3 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 8) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (right: 17; left: 0; other: 2; 2 patients "atypical": unclear whether L or mixed) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (CIAT group: mean 60.2 ± 48.9 months; untreated group: mean 41.9 ± 30.0 months; all > 1 year) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | TT, PPVT, BNT, semantic fluency, phonemic fluency, communicative activities log |
Aphasia severity | 6 mild (2 control, 4 CIAT); 5 moderate (3 control, 2 CIAT); 8 severe (3 control, 5 CIAT) |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~3 weeks later; T3: 3 months after the end of treatment |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | CIAT, 4 hours/day, 5 days/week, 2 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | No (Philips 3 Tesla or Siemens 3 Tesla; models not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 600 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Tone decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Appear mismatched at least in healthy controls in Table 3 |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Lateralized frontal, temporal, and parietal |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation |
Control condition | Finger tapping |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control data in Szaflarski et al. (2008); frontal activation L-lateralized, temporal less so |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T2 (n = 11) vs untreated T2 (n = 8) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L brainstem ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R superior parietal |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 4 |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T3 (n = 11) vs untreated T3 (n = 8) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, no test |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R orbitofrontal ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 4 |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T2 (n = 11) vs untreated T2 (n = 8) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L precuneus ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R posterior STS ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | Based on coordinates in Table 4 |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T3 (n = 11) vs untreated T3 (n = 8) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R basal ganglia ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ R posterior STS ↓ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T1 (n = 11) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R anterior temporal ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T2 (n = 11) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T3 (n = 11) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ L hippocampus/MTL ↑ R superior parietal ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated T1 (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L angular gyrus ↓ L mid temporal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R angular gyrus ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated T2 (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R mid temporal ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L orbitofrontal ↓ L intraparietal sulcus ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ L brainstem ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R inferior parietal lobule ↓ R supramarginal gyrus ↓ R anterior temporal ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R hippocampus/MTL |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated T3 (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but not significantly for the semantic decision task, and more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R superior parietal ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L orbitofrontal ↓ L mid temporal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L posterior cingulate ↓ L cerebellum ↓ L hippocampus/MTL ↓ R angular gyrus ↓ R anterior temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T1 (n = 11) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L superior parietal ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T2 (n = 11) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L anterior cingulate ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ L occipital ↓ R IFG pars orbitalis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia CIAT T3 (n = 11) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ L posterior cingulate ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated T1 (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L occipital ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ L cerebellum ↓ R superior parietal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated T2 (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG ↑ R posterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↓ L occipital ↓ R superior parietal ↓ R occipital ↓ R cerebellum |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated T3 (n = 8) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L occipital ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R occipital ↑ R cerebellum ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L occipital ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R cerebellum ↓ R basal ganglia |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R brainstem ↑ R basal ganglia |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | Δ BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior MTG ↑ R thalamus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R mid temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | Δ BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 50 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R orbitofrontal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia ANOVA including T1, T2, T3 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) cerebellar LI; (4) fronto-parietal LI; (5) Broca's LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia CIAT (n = 11) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) vs (untreated (n = 8) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) cerebellar LI; (4) fronto-parietal LI; (5) Broca's LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia ANOVA including T1, T2, T3 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) cerebellar LI; (4) fronto-parietal LI; (5) Broca's LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia CIAT (n = 11) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) vs (untreated (n = 8) T1 ≠ T2 ≠ T3) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) cerebellar LI; (4) fronto-parietal LI; (5) Broca's LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | Mandarin |
Inclusion criteria | Broca's aphasia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 10 |
Number of control participants | 10 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 55.9 ± 13.4 years, range 40-70 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 7; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 10; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 1-3 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB |
Aphasia severity | Moderate-severe |
Aphasia type | All Broca's |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Not at all |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (GE Signa 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (only three pictures were named per 30-second block) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 186 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | No (not described) |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (no description of model fitting) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (not described) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Somewhat L-lateralized frontal and anterior temporal language activations, but the majority of activation is in unexpected regions |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on arbitrary cluster extent |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 10 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | In the footnote to Table 2, there is a reference to FWE correction with Monte Carlo simulation, but this is not described in the text, and the values in the table appear to be inconsistent with that |
Findings | ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L occipital ↑ L thalamus ↑ R inferior parietal lobule ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R precuneus ↑ R anterior temporal ↓ L IFG ↓ L orbitofrontal ↓ L somato-motor ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction |
Findings notes | Findings are based on coordinates, which in many cases do not match the labels assigned in the paper |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Able to name 20% of pictures correctly in the scanner |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 32 (plus 14 excluded: < 20% accuracy in scanner) |
Number of control participants | 25 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (29 of the participants overlap with the other Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017) paper) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 58.8 ± 8.6 years, range 45.7-78.2 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 19; females: 12; stated to be not matched, but difference not significant) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 26; left: 3; other: 2) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 40.9 ± 36.1 months, 4.9-151.0 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, PNT |
Aphasia severity | AQ mean 77.7 ± 21.0, range 22.8-99.2 |
Aphasia type | 21 anomic, 7 Broca's, 3 conduction, 1 transcortical sensory |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Mean 27.5 ± 22.9 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (total images acquired not stated; separation of adjacent events (covert and overt naming) will be limited because of the small amount of jitter in their timing (only 1500 ms)) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | ~450 but not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (entire phases where picture was displayed modeled as covert and overt naming; difficult to separate phases due to timing) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (silently name, correct trials) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral frontal and occipito-temporal, but not posterior temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral frontal and occipito-temporal, but not posterior temporal; speech motor activation not readily apparent |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (both phases, correct trials) |
Control condition | Picture naming (both phases, incorrect trials) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Unknown |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Unknown |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | N/A |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | N/A |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | N/A |
Control activation notes | Control data N/A because controls do not typically make errors |
Contrast notes | It is unclear whether there were no-response trials and whether they were modeled as incorrect |
First level contrast | Picture naming (silently name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain gray matter |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | Threshold of z > 3.1 mentioned in results, but presume 2.3 based on methods and figure |
Findings | ↑ R precuneus ↓ L occipital |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain gray matter |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | Threshold of z > 3.1 mentioned in results, but presume 2.3 based on methods and figure |
Findings | ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ L orbitofrontal ↑ L precuneus ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R orbitofrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R posterior STS ↓ L IFG ↓ L insula ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L intraparietal sulcus ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L hippocampus/MTL ↓ R intraparietal sulcus |
Findings notes | Labels based largely on text with some adjustments based on figures; overall pattern of decreased L activity and increased R activity is quite convincing |
First level contrast | Picture naming (silently name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain gray matter |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L anterior temporal ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L supramarginal gyrus ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R somato-motor |
Findings notes | L anterior temporal correlation remained significant after accounting for lesion load and other factors |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain gray matter |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STG ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior STS ↑ R occipital ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L angular gyrus |
Findings notes | L IFG pars orbitalis, R pSTS, and R somato-motor correlations remained remained significant after accounting for lesion load and other factors; note that the pars orbitalis region is described as frontal pole in the paper but the coordinates and image support pars orbitalis |
First level contrast | Picture naming (both phases, correct trials) vs picture naming (both phases, incorrect trials) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional performance-defined conditions |
Group(s) | Aphasia with naming < 80% (n = 24) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain gray matter |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | ~.01 (z > 2.3) |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 11 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) right IPS; (2) left IPS; (3) left PTr; (4) left dPOp; (5) right superior motor cortex; (6) right ventral motor cortex; (7) right supramarginal sulcus; (8) left medial SMA; (9) right marginal sulcus; (10) left dorsal motor cortex; (11) right STS |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions that were activated for control > aphasia (ROIs 1-4) or aphasia > control (ROIs 5-11) |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Familywise error (FWE) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R posterior STS ↓ L IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | The L IFG pars opercularis and the R posterior STS also contributed to predicting PNT scores even when lesion load on critical areas for picture naming, and several other variables, were included in multiple regression models |
First level contrast | Picture naming (silently name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L anterior temporal |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activity for covert naming correlated with naming ability in patients, after controlling for lesion and demographic factors |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L frontal pole; (2) R postcentral gyrus; (3) R STS |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activity for overt naming correlated with naming ability in patients, after controlling for lesion and demographic factors |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion patterns identified with SVR-LSM |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | SVR-LSM was used to identify regions of damage associated with activation of R pSTS ROI (defined based on SPM analysis 2). The results were thresholded at voxelwise p < .01 (CDT), cluster extent > 500 voxels. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Damage to the L IFG pars opercularis was associated with more activity in the R pSTS. Damage to the L pSTS was associated with less activity in the R pSTS. |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 26) |
Covariate | Lesion patterns identified with SVR-LSM |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | SVR-LSM was used to identify regions of damage associated with activation of L IFG pars opercularis ROI (defined based on SPM analysis 2). The results were thresholded at voxelwise p < .01 (CDT), cluster extent > 500 voxels. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Damage to the L pSTG, L pSTS, and white matter underlying the L precuneus was associated with more activity in the L IFG pars opercularis. There were no regions associated with less activity. |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | 10% accuracy on scanner task |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 39 (plus 10 excluded: < 10% accuracy in scanner) |
Number of control participants | 37 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (29 of the participants overlap with the other Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017) paper) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 59.8 ± 10.0 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 26; females: 13) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 33; left: 4; other: 2; missing for 2 participants) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 52.9 ± 51.4 months, range 6.3-255.7 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, PNT |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | 23 anomic, 11 Broca's, 3 conduction, 1 transcortical sensory, 1 Wernicke's |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (total images acquired not stated; separation of adjacent events (covert and overt naming) will be limited because of the small amount of jitter in their timing (only 1500 ms)) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | ~450 but not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (not stated but see Skipper-Kallal et al. (2017b)) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | at each voxel, individuals with lesions to that voxel were excluded from analysis |
Language condition | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral frontal and occipito-temporal, but not posterior temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Bilateral frontal and occipito-temporal, but not posterior temporal; speech motor activation not readily apparent |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L cerebellum ↑ L thalamus ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R insula ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R basal ganglia ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L orbitofrontal ↓ L intraparietal sulcus ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | Based on Table 2 |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R posterior MTG ↑ R Heschl's gyrus ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ L mid temporal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L cerebellum ↓ L thalamus ↓ L hippocampus/MTL |
Findings notes | Based on Table 3 |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ L occipital ↑ L basal ganglia ↑ R IFG ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R occipital ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R brainstem ↑ R basal ganglia |
Findings notes | Based on Table 4, except for R frontal activations which are missing from the table, and were added based on the figure |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L somato-motor ↑ L precuneus ↑ L occipital ↑ L cerebellum ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R insula ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R mid temporal ↑ R occipital ↑ R cerebellum ↑ R basal ganglia ↑ R hippocampus/MTL |
Findings notes | Based on Table 4, except for bilateral occipital activations which are missing from the table, and were added based on the figure |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IPS damage (n not stated) vs without IPS damage (n not stated) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with insula damage (n = 18) vs without insula damage (n = 21) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 16) vs without IFG POp damage (n = 23) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with motor cortex damage (n = 24) vs without motor cortex damage (n = 15) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with STS damage (n not stated) vs without STS damage (n not stated) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and lenient voxelwise p |
Software | FSL 5.0.6 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 16) |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R DLPFC |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak location for decreased activation for patients with left insula and left POp lesions compared to patients without said damage |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia without IFG POp damage (n = 23) |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R DLPFC |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak location for decreased activation for patients with left insula and left POp lesions compared to patients without said damage |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with insula damage (n = 18) |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R DLPFC |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak location for decreased activation for patients with left insula and left POp lesions compared to patients without said damage |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia without insula damage (n = 21) |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R DLPFC |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Peak location for decreased activation for patients with left insula and left POp lesions compared to patients without said damage |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IPS damage (n not stated) vs without IPS damage (n not stated) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IPS; (2) L insula; (3) L IFG pars opercularis; (4) R IPS; (5) R insula |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | 5 mm spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with insula damage (n = 18) vs without insula damage (n = 21) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IPS; (2) L insula; (3) L IFG pars opercularis; (4) R IPS; (5) R insula |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | 5 mm spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (prepare to name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with IFG POp damage (n = 16) vs without IFG POp damage (n = 23) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Covert phase but accuracy derived from overt phase |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 5 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IPS; (2) L insula; (3) L IFG pars opercularis; (4) R IPS; (5) R insula |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | 5 mm spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with motor cortex damage (n = 24) vs without motor cortex damage (n = 15) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L motor; (2) L pSTS; (3) R motor; (4) R pSTS |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | 5 mm spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↑ R somato-motor |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with STS damage (n not stated) vs without STS damage (n not stated) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L motor; (2) L pSTS; (3) R motor; (4) R pSTS |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | 5 mm spheres around control peaks |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↓ R somato-motor |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia without motor cortex damage (n = 15) |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R motor |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | 5 mm sphere around control peak |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (produce the name, correct trials) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with motor cortex damage (n = 24) |
Covariate | PNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, correct trials only |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R motor |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | 5 mm sphere around control peak |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Lesion volume covariate |
Findings | ↑ R somato-motor |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 12 (plus 2 excluded: 1 for illness; 1 for MRI contraindication or personal conflict (inconsistent information provided)) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (same data as Dietz et al. (2016), which is a methodological paper) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (AAC group: range 39-63 years; usual care group: range 47-71 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 7) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 11; left: 1) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (AAC group: range 16-170 months; usual care group: range 38-105 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB, Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia |
Aphasia severity | AAC group: AQ range 37.6-82.4; usual care group: AQ range 36.7-89.2 |
Aphasia type | AAC group: 2 Broca's, 1 anomic, 1 conduction, 1 global, 1 Wernicke's; usual care group: 2 anomic, 2 Broca's, 1 conduction, 1 Wernicke's |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | AAC group: range 7849-30570 voxels; usual care group: 1583-30110 voxels (voxel size not stated) |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~4 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | AAC group: treatment aimed at teaching participants how to utilize AAC to facilitate discourse; usual care group: traditional SLT, not focused on discourse or AAC specifically |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 135 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No (no description of HRF model, which is important given sparse sampling design) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | additional methodological details in Dietz et al. (2016) |
Language condition | Verb generation (overt) |
Control condition | Noun repetition |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control data in Allendorfer et al. (2012); somewhat L-lateralized frontal, temporal and parietal activations, but also extensive midline activation |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation (overt) vs noun repetition |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with AAC treatment (n = 6) T2 vs usual care T2 (n = 6) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (marginal treatment effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Frontal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Temporal LI calculated but not reported |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation (overt) vs noun repetition |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (both groups) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ WAB AQ |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (gain in AQ not tested for significance) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Frontal LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | Temporal LI calculated but not reported |
Findings | ↑ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | Semantic aphasia; left frontal damage (+ other regions, typically) |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 14 |
Number of control participants | 16 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 61 ± 11 years, range 38-80 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 9) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 11-264 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | Cambridge semantic battery, three additional semantic tasks, connected speech words per minute, repetition from PALPA |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | 6 anomic, 2 Broca's, 2 global, 2 transcortical sensory, 1 mixed transcortical, 1 not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L IFG plus other MCA regions; vATL and pMTG spared |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (GE Signa HDx 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 348 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | interleaved silent steady state imaging |
Language condition | Listening to high or low ambiguity sentences |
Control condition | Listening to spectrally rotated speech |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Hard to evaluate contrast because a "semantic mask" is used but is not described in detail |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to high ambiguity sentences |
Control condition | Listening to low ambiguity sentences |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to high or low ambiguity sentences vs listening to spectrally rotated speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L vATL; (2) L pMTG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Functional coordinates in literature |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | ANOVA revealed main effect of group (patient vs control), confirmed in follow-up tests for each ROI |
Findings | ↑ L posterior MTG ↑ L anterior temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to high ambiguity sentences vs listening to low ambiguity sentences |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L vATL; (2) L pMTG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Functional coordinates in literature |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | No interaction of group by condition |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to high ambiguity sentences vs listening to low ambiguity sentences |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia (subset with resting state data, n = 10) vs control (subset with resting state data, n = 10) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A whole brain analysis was carried out to identify regions where the groups differed in the extent to which the strength of functional connectivity at rest from L pMTG was associated with the difference in signal between the high ambiguity and low ambiguity conditions in the same ROI. Thresholding is not described and cluster extent is not reported. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a functional activation by group interaction in the L aSTG. For controls, there was a positive association between L pMTG activity and functional connectivity to aSTG, while for the patients, there was a negative association. |
First level contrast | Listening to high ambiguity sentences vs listening to low ambiguity sentences |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia (subset with resting state data, n = 10) vs control (subset with resting state data, n = 10) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A whole brain analysis was carried out to identify regions where the groups differed in the extent to which the strength of functional connectivity at rest from L pMTG was associated with the difference in signal between the high ambiguity and low ambiguity conditions in the same ROI. Thresholding is not described. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | No interaction is reported; both groups showed a correlation between L vATL activity and functional connectivity to a ventral IFG region |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Aphasia at acute screening (not necessarily at first study time point) |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 17 (plus 1 excluded: significant signal artifacts) |
Number of control participants | 85 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 46 ± 16 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 9; females: 8) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (right: 17; left: 0; all patients stated to be right handed, but "ambidextrous patients" mentioned on p. 364) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (T1: ~2 weeks; T2: ~6 weeks; T3: ~12 weeks; T4: ~26 weeks; T5: ~52 weeks) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | PPVT, BNT, phonemic fluency, semantic fluency, complex ideation subtest of BDAE |
Aphasia severity | Not stated for study timepoints, but on admission, aphasia severity was assessed with the TT: 2 no aphasia per cutoff but clinical impression of aphasia, 5 mild, 6 moderate, 4 severe |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | No |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA; mostly posterior per Supplementary Figure 2 |
Participants notes | Presence and severity of aphasia assessed on hospital admission, not at first study time point, so it is not clear that all participants actually had aphasia at first study time point |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: ~2 weeks; T2: ~6 weeks; T3: ~12 weeks; T4: ~26 weeks; T5: ~52 weeks |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Not stated |
Is the scanner described? | No (Philips 3 Tesla or Siemens 3 Tesla; models not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 600 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | scanner identity appropriately included as covariate |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Tone decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L lateral and medial frontal and AG, strongly lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Verb generation |
Control condition | Finger tapping |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L lateral and medial frontal and mid temporal, strongly lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L Heschl's gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T4 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T5 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Semantic decision accuracy |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L anterior temporal |
Findings notes | Unclear why this type of analysis was run only for semantic task, and only at T1 |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Δ BDAE complex ideation subtest |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Δ semantic fluency |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R anterior temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T4 vs aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Δ BDAE complex ideation subtest |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise correction based on permutation testing |
Software | SPM12/SnPM13 |
Voxelwise p | FWE p < .05 |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia (comparisons between all pairs of time points) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia (comparisons between all pairs of time points) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T4 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T5 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Patients less accurate than controls on both tasks, but more so on the tone decision task |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ LI (language network) ↓ LI (frontal) |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T4 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia T5 vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) frontal LI; (2) temporo-parietal LI; (3) language network LI; (4) cerebellar LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia (comparisons between all pairs of time points) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear similar |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | PPI analyses were carried out to investigate potential changes over time in how connectivity from L and R IFG was modulated by the semantic decision task. The resultant SPM was thresholded at FWE p < .05 using permutation testing implemented in SnPM 13. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Verb generation vs finger tapping |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia (comparisons between all pairs of time points) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | PPI analyses were carried out to investigate potential changes over time in how connectivity from L and R IFG was modulated by the verb generation task. The resultant SPM was thresholded at FWE p < .05 using permutation testing implemented in SnPM 13. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Residual phonologic retrieval deficit; intact semantic processing |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 21 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 56.4 ± 12.5 years, range 30-80 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 10) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 21; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 1134 ± 1491 days, range 180-6732 days) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | Pseudoword rhyme matching, semantic picture matching (similar to PPT-P), picture naming |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Mean 73.4 ± 58.6 cc, range 6.7-227.0 cc |
Lesion location | 17 L MCA, 2 combined L MCA/ACA, combined 2 L MCA/PCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (GE Excite 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No (precise timing of stimuli not stated; total images acquired not stated) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Reading nouns aloud (correct trials) |
Control condition | Reading nouns aloud (incorrect trials) |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | N/A |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | N/A |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | N/A |
Control activation notes | Control data N/A because controls do not typically make errors |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Reading nouns aloud (correct trials) vs reading nouns aloud (incorrect trials) |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional performance-defined conditions |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, by design |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | AFNI |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 1.609 cc |
Statistical details | Regarding correction for multiple comparisons, addition of monoexponential function reduces but does not eliminate inflation of p values (Cox et al., 2017) |
Findings | ↑ L angular gyrus ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R insula ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | Positive region (L AG) was part of the semantic network, while many negative regions were positively modulated by reaction time in the aphasia group |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 12 (plus 1 excluded: scanned at only 2 out of 3 time points) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 26-66 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 9; females: 3) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 11; left: 1) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 1-12 years) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, semantic fluency, phonemic fluency |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 10.4-94.6 |
Aphasia type | 8 anomic, 2 Broca's, 1 conduction, 1 global |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic (1-2 weeks prior to treatment); T2: post-treatment (within 1 week after end of 2-week treatment); T3: 13-20 weeks after end of treatment |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Modified CIAT + intermittent theta burst stimulation to residual left hemispheric language activation, 45 minutes/session, 5 days/week, 2 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Allegra 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 330 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Tone decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L frontal and temporal, plus other semantic regions |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 0.928 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L Heschl's gyrus ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R superior parietal ↑ R precuneus ↑ R mid temporal ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ L dorsal precentral ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L somato-motor ↓ L superior parietal ↓ L occipital |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 0.928 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STS ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L anterior temporal ↓ L anterior cingulate ↓ R IFG ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R somato-motor ↓ R precuneus ↓ R posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ R anterior temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 0.928 cc |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L supramarginal gyrus ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L precuneus ↑ L posterior STG ↑ L mid temporal ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ L posterior cingulate ↓ L somato-motor ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Δ WAB AQ |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 0.928 cc |
Statistical details | Inclusive mask of voxels that differed between T2 and T3 |
Findings | ↓ L inferior parietal lobule |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs tone decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Δ BNT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on cluster_threshold_beta |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .05 |
Cluster extent | 0.928 cc |
Statistical details | Inclusive mask of voxels that differed between T1 and T3 |
Findings | ↓ R IFG |
Findings notes | — |
Language | Dutch |
Inclusion criteria | Severe non-fluent aphasia (< 50 words/minute); articulation deficits; repetition severely affected; moderate-good auditory comprehension |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 9 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (subacute: mean 51.2 years, range 25-61 years; chronic: mean 54.0 years, range 21-66 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 5; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0; other: 1) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (subacute: range 0.5-3 months; chronic: range 17-40 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT, ANELT |
Aphasia severity | T1: subacute: ASRS median 1, range 0-2; ANELT range 10-29; chronic: ASRS median 1.5, range 1-2; ANELT range 20-29; T2: subacute: ASRS range 1-3; ANELT range 10-43; chronic: ASRS range 1-2; ANELT range 22-31 |
Aphasia type | T1: all severe non-fluent; T2: not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Extent and location |
Lesion extent | Subacute: range 32.4-141.2 cc (no lesion extent was reported for one subacute participant because there was no tissue loss yet); chronic: range 27.4-87.9 cc |
Lesion location | 8 L MCA, 1 L SMA and R insular-temporoparietal |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—mixed |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre treatment/subacute or chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~6 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | MIT, 5+ hours/week |
Is the scanner described? | No (GE 3 Tesla; model not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 132 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to narrative speech |
Control condition | Listening to reversed speech |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activations that were "not clearly related to known language areas" were excluded, but the basis for this determination is not clear |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activations that were "not clearly related to known language areas" were excluded, but the basis for this determination is not clear |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ AAT repetition score |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activations that were "not clearly related to known language areas" were excluded, but the basis for this determination is not clear |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to narrative speech vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ ANELT |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Laterality indi(ces) |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | Language network LI |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Activations that were "not clearly related to known language areas" were excluded, but the basis for this determination is not clear |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | Dutch |
Inclusion criteria | MRS ≤ 3; ability to perform tasks |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 12 |
Number of control participants | 8 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 67.9 ± 11.4 years, range 46-86 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 10; females: 2) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 12; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No* (moderate limitation) (T1: within 2 weeks; T2: ~3 months; T3: ~6 months; T4: ~12 months; specific timing of first time point not stated) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | AAT, BNT |
Aphasia severity | T1: 8 moderate, 2 severe, 2 not stated; T2: 4 moderate, 3 recovered, 2 not stated, 1 mild, 1 severe |
Aphasia type | T1: 6 Broca's, 3 anomic, 2 Wernicke's, 1 global; T2: 4 anomic, 3 recovered, 2 Broca's, 1 unclassified, 1 Wernicke's |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 9-208 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: within 2 weeks; T2: ~3 months; T3: ~6 months; T4: ~12 months; specific timing of first time point not stated |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Not stated |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Philips Achieva 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (stimulus presentation was self-paced, but the ITI is not reported, nor are the number of trials presented per condition; it is likely that the language and control blocks contained different numbers of trials) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 1656 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | not all participants scanned at each time point; the number scanned at each time point is not stated |
Language condition | Written word-picture matching |
Control condition | Visual decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Primarily bilateral visual activations; frontal activation is L-lateralized |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Visual decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | L frontal, L posterior ITG, L superior parietal |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T1 (n = 10) |
Covariate | Subsequent outcome (T4) overall language measure (average of AAT measures) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 12 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | Activation predicted later outcome even when initial language performance was included in the model |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (all time points) |
Covariate | Overall language measure (average of AAT measures) all time points |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 12 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Mixed model; minimal detail provided |
Findings | ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (all time points) |
Covariate | Average of AAT comprehension score and BNT, all time points |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 12 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Mixed model; minimal detail provided |
Findings | ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (all time points) |
Covariate | Picture-word matching accuracy, all time points |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 12 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Mixed model; minimal detail provided |
Findings | ↑ R posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Written word-picture matching vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia: linear effect of time |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 12 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) bilateral dorsal anterior cingulate; (2) L angular gyrus; (3) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (4) L thalamus; (5) L MFG; (6) L posterior ITG; (7) R angular gyrus; (8) R IFG pars triangularis; (9) R thalamus; (10) R posterior ITG; (11) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (12) R MFG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Mixed model; minimal detail provided |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R anterior cingulate ↑ R thalamus ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | Similar numbers of findings are reported for controls |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (subset who returned for follow-up) T1 (n = 10) |
Covariate | Subsequent outcome (T4) overall language measure (average of AAT measures) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (not appropriate to correlate T1 imaging with T4 behavior without T1 behavior in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (all time points) |
Covariate | Overall language measure (average of AAT measures) all time points |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Mixed model; minimal detail provided |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (all time points) |
Covariate | Average of AAT comprehension score and BNT, all time points |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Mixed model; minimal detail provided |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia (all time points) |
Covariate | Semantic decision accuracy, all time points |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Mixed model; minimal detail provided |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs visual decision |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia: linear effect of time |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 6 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L angular gyrus; (2) L IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (3) L posterior ITG; (4) R angular gyrus; (5) R IFG pars opercularis and triangularis; (6) R posterior ITG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Control activations and their homotopic counterparts in the R hemisphere; activation measured as count of voxels activated at p < 0.001, uncorrected |
Correction for multiple comparisons | False discovery rate (FDR) |
Statistical details | Mixed model; minimal detail provided |
Findings | ↑ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus ↑ R angular gyrus ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | Similar numbers of findings are reported for controls |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 18 (plus 1 excluded: developed a hematoma between baseline and post-testing) |
Number of control participants | 23 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (range 22-73 years; controls were younger) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 7) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (right: 15; left: 3; not stated for controls) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 13-107 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | WAB, Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS), Northwestern Naming Battery (NNB), analysis of spontaneous speech (Cinderella story) using Northwestern Narrative Language Analysis (NNLA) protocol |
Aphasia severity | AQ range 52.8-91.7 |
Aphasia type | Not stated, except that "language deficits were consistent with nonfluent aphasia and agrammatism" |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | Mostly L MCA but some lesions include PCA or ACA territory |
Participants notes | One patient had two strokes within one day, but we would consider that essentially a single stroke |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~12 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | 13 patients were treated and 5 were not; treatment of underlying forms; 90 minutes/session, 2 sessions/week until 80% accuracy met on weekly probe task, then 1 session/week, 12 weeks except for one patient who demonstrated rapid improvement and completed treatment in 6 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla or Siemens Prisma 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (stimulus timing described does not match stated duration of data acquisition; timing of language and control trials not matched) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | ~482 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | 2 runs before treatment and 2 runs after treatment; each pair of runs took place on two separate days (1-7 days apart) |
Language condition | Auditory sentence-picture verification |
Control condition | Listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | L-lateralized inferior frontal and posterior temporal, but also bilateral posterior inferior temporal and lateral occipital activations |
Contrast notes | Contrast described as "passive > control" but seems to involve active and passive sentences |
First level contrast | Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated (n = 13) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Out-of-scanner performance on passive sentences improved |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 37 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L precuneus ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R somato-motor ↑ R supramarginal gyrus ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R superior parietal ↑ R precuneus |
Findings notes | Based on Table 7 and Figure 8 |
First level contrast | Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia natural history (n = 5) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | SPM8 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | 37 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia treated (n=13) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia natural history (n=5) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L hemisphere sentence processing network (IFGpt, pMTG, pSTG, AG); (2) R hemisphere homotopic regions; (3) L dorsal attention network (MFG, PrCG, SPL, sLOC); (4) R dorsal attention network (same regions) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Sentence processing network based on Walenski et al. (2019); dorsal attention network based on Corbetta et al. (2008) and Vincent et al. (2008); ROIs were defined based on Harvard-Oxford atlas which would align imperfectly with these functional networks; dependent variable was number of active voxels (p < .001, uncorrected) divided by number of intact voxels |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Derivation of dependent measures from ROIs difficulty to follow, but it seems that ROIs with less than 5 voxels upregulated were excluded and deactivations were not considered, meaning that estimates of change may be biased |
Findings | ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L dorsal precentral ↑ L angular gyrus ↑ L intraparietal sulcus ↑ L superior parietal ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R superior parietal |
Findings notes | Bilateral dorsal attention network; findings were for networks as a whole; regions coded correspond to atlas ROIs |
First level contrast | Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | ∆ offline comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L hemisphere sentence processing network (IFGpt, pMTG, pSTG, AG); (2) R hemisphere homotopic regions; (3) L dorsal attention network (MFG, PrCG, SPL, sLOC); (4) R dorsal attention network (same regions) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Sentence processing network based on Walenski et al. (2019); dorsal attention network based on Corbetta et al. (2008) and Vincent et al. (2008); ROIs were defined based on Harvard-Oxford atlas which would align imperfectly with these functional networks; dependent variable was number of active voxels (p < .001, uncorrected) divided by number of intact voxels |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Derivation of dependent measures from ROIs difficulty to follow, but it seems that ROIs with less than 5 voxels upregulated were excluded and deactivations were not considered, meaning that estimates of change may be biased |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R superior parietal ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Findings notes | R homotopic sentence processing network and R dorsal attention network; findings were for networks as a whole; regions coded correspond to atlas ROIs |
First level contrast | Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia participants with eye tracking data (n = 16) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | ∆ decrease in eye tracking online thematic prediction score |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L hemisphere sentence processing network (IFGpt, pMTG, pSTG, AG); (2) R hemisphere homotopic regions; (3) L dorsal attention network (MFG, PrCG, SPL, sLOC); (4) R dorsal attention network (same regions) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Sentence processing network based on Walenski et al. (2019); dorsal attention network based on Corbetta et al. (2008) and Vincent et al. (2008); ROIs were defined based on Harvard-Oxford atlas which would align imperfectly with these functional networks; dependent variable was number of active voxels (p < .001, uncorrected) divided by number of intact voxels |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Derivation of dependent measures from ROIs difficulty to follow, but it seems that ROIs with less than 5 voxels upregulated were excluded and deactivations were not considered, meaning that estimates of change may be biased |
Findings | ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Findings notes | R homotopic sentence processing network; findings were for networks as a whole; regions coded correspond to atlas ROIs |
First level contrast | Auditory sentence-picture verification vs listening to reversed speech and viewing scrambled pictures |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia participants with eye tracking data (n = 16) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | ∆ eye tracking online thematic integragration score |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L hemisphere sentence processing network (IFGpt, pMTG, pSTG, AG); (2) R hemisphere homotopic regions; (3) L dorsal attention network (MFG, PrCG, SPL, sLOC); (4) R dorsal attention network (same regions) |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Sentence processing network based on Walenski et al. (2019); dorsal attention network based on Corbetta et al. (2008) and Vincent et al. (2008); ROIs were defined based on Harvard-Oxford atlas which would align imperfectly with these functional networks; dependent variable was number of active voxels (p < .001, uncorrected) divided by number of intact voxels |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Derivation of dependent measures from ROIs difficulty to follow, but it seems that ROIs with less than 5 voxels upregulated were excluded and deactivations were not considered, meaning that estimates of change may be biased |
Findings | ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ R ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ R dorsal precentral ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R intraparietal sulcus ↑ R superior parietal |
Findings notes | R dorsal attention network; findings were for networks as a whole; regions coded correspond to atlas ROIs |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Anomia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 30 (plus 5 excluded: 2 withdrew from non-treatment arm; 3 fMRI acquisition errors; 1 did not complete treatment and post-treatment scanning (but of these latter 4, one must have at least completed the non-treatment arm)) |
Number of control participants | 17 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (treated group: mean 62.8 ± 10.2 years, range 42-80 years; untreated group: mean 59.0 ± 11.8 years, range 39-79 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 21; females: 9) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 27; left: 3) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (treated group: mean 58.3 ± 51.8 months, range 12-170 months; untreated group: mean 85.2 ± 141.9 months, range 10-467 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | WAB, BNT, PPT |
Aphasia severity | Treated group: AQ mean 60.1 ± 24.0, range 11.7-95.2; untreated group: AQ mean 65.8 ± 24.6, range 26.9-91.5 |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Treated group: 136.6 ± 81.1 cc, range 11.7-317.1 cc; untreated group: 112.7 ± 94.6 cc, range 1.6-317.1 cc |
Lesion location | Mostly MCA with a few extending into PCA |
Participants notes | There were 26 patients in the treated group and 10 in the untreated group, but 6 patients overlapped between the two groups (they joined the treated group after completing the untreated phase) |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, ~12 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Semantic naming treatment, 2 sessions/week |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla, except for 2 patients on a Siemens Prisma 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (total images not stated; short ITI and minimal jitter) |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | not stated |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (unclear whether there was sufficient resting data to allow the key contrast to be computed) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Picture naming (trained items) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | No |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Most ROIs deactivated in controls |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated T1 (n = 26) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 16 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFGorb; (2) L IFGtri; (3) L IFGop; (4) L MFG; (5) L PrCG; (6) L MTG; (7) L SMG; (8) L AG; (9-16) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL but lesioned voxels were excluded from ROIs on an individual basis |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ L angular gyrus |
Findings notes | Significant interaction of ROI by group |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated T2 (n = 26) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 16 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFGorb; (2) L IFGtri; (3) L IFGop; (4) L MFG; (5) L PrCG; (6) L MTG; (7) L SMG; (8) L AG; (9-16) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL but lesioned voxels were excluded from ROIs on an individual basis |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ R IFG pars opercularis ↑ R IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | Significant interaction of ROI by group; patients also showed more activity than controls across the average of all ROIs |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia untreated (n = 10) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Anatomical |
How many ROIs are there? | 16 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFGorb; (2) L IFGtri; (3) L IFGop; (4) L MFG; (5) L PrCG; (6) L MTG; (7) L SMG; (8) L AG; (9-16) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | AAL but lesioned voxels were excluded from ROIs on an individual basis |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | No main effect of time or interaction of time by ROI |
First level contrast | Picture naming (trained items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia treated (n = 26) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear model was constructed to examine the relationship between proportion of spared tissue in each L hemisphere ROI and changes in activation over time. The model is not described in sufficient detail. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant 3-way interaction of time by ROI by spared tissue, such that in some regions (AG, MFG, IFG orb, SMG), less spared tissue was associated with greater increases in activation, while in others (PrCG, IFG op, IFG tri), less spared tissue was associated with greater decreases in activation. |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | < 80% on PNT; able to name at least 5 out of 40 items during fMRI; WAB-R spontaneous speech ≥ 2; WAB-R auditory comprehension ≥ 2 |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 87 |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (65 were previously included in Fridriksson et al. (2018), a tDCS study) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (typical BDNF genotype group mean 59.6 ± 11.2 years, range 29-77 years; atypical BDNF genotype group mean 57.7 ± 10.9 years, range 30-76 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 58; females: 29) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 87; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (typical BDNF genotype group: mean 44.0 ± 38.7 months; atypical BDNF genotype group: mean 34.5 ± 36.9 months; all participants > 6 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity and type |
Language evaluation | WAB, PNT, PPT |
Aphasia severity | Typical BDNF genotype group: AQ mean 64.2 ± 20.3; atypical BDNF genotype group: AQ mean 54.3 ± 21.0 |
Aphasia type | Typical BDNF genotype group: 25 Broca's, 12 anomic, 11 conduction, 2 transcortical motor aphasia, 2 Wernicke's, 1 global; atypical BDNF genotype group: 16 Broca's, 6 anomic, 6 conduction, 3 global, 3 Wernicke's |
First stroke only? | No |
Stroke type | Mixed etiologies |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Typical BDNF genotype group: 121.4 ± 73.2 cc; atypical BDNF genotype group: 142.2 ± 88.4 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla or Siemens Prisma 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 60 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling |
Language condition | Picture naming |
Control condition | Viewing abstract pictures |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Picture naming vs viewing abstract pictures |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with typical genotype (n = 53) vs atypical genotype (n = 34) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Voxelwise FWE correction |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | US English |
Inclusion criteria | Chronic dysgraphia (acquired impairment in spelling) |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 21 (plus 4 excluded: 3 health reasons; 1 data acquisition error) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 40-80 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 13; females: 8) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 16; left: 3; other: 2) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 14-209 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | Spelling (PALPA 40 and 54, and other word lists), oral reading (PALPA 35), reading comprehension (PALPA 51), spoken word-picture matching and picture naming tests from Northwestern Naming Battery, PPT-P; note no generic aphasia battery, but fairly complete coverage of language domains |
Aphasia severity | Spelling of untrained items range 51%-94% |
Aphasia type | 4 orthographic working memory deficit, 8 orthographic long-term memory deficit, 9 both types of deficit |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 7.7-215.0 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA with L ventral occipitotemporal cortex mostly intact |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1: pre-treatment/chronic; T2: post-treatment, 6-24 weeks later |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Spelling treatment, 60-80 minutes/day, 2 days/week, range 6-24 weeks |
Is the scanner described? | No (not stated) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 1232 (four runs distributed over two days) |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (cerebellum excluded) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (not feasible to separate closely spaced instruction, word, and letter/response, especially when responses will be compared to rest) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Spelling probe (training items) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Task comes from Rapp and Lipka (2011), who report lateralized activations for the contrast of spelling probes to case verification, but do not report results relative to fixation baseline |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Spelling probe (known items) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | Task comes from Rapp and Lipka (2011), who report lateralized activations for the contrast of spelling probes to case verification, but do not report results relative to fixation baseline |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Behavioral data notes | See section S2, but main effects include known items also |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Appears to be restricted to voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction based on 3dClustSim |
Software | BrainVoyager QX 2.4 or SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .01 |
Cluster extent | 49 voxels (size not stated) |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L posterior cingulate ↑ R angular gyrus ↑ R posterior cingulate |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ spelling accuracy on training items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R AG; (2) L PCC; (3) R PCC |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions activated in SPM analysis 1 |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 3 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) R AG; (2) L PCC; (3) R PCC |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Regions activated in SPM analysis 1 |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ spelling accuracy on training items (T2 vs T1) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ventral occipitotemporal cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The region that showed an increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints T1 (n = 20) |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items (T2 vs T1) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ventral occipitotemporal cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The region that showed an increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ spelling accuracy on training items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ventral occipitotemporal cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The region that showed an increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | L ventral occipitotemporal cortex |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The region that showed an increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Appear mismatched |
Behavioral data notes | See section S2, where Figures S1 and S2 appear to show differences; the main effects of time were not significant for accuracy or RT, but those analyses included known items also, which had smaller effects |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Local Heterogeneity Regression Analysis (Local-Hreg) was used to identify brain regions where the heterogeneity of timecourses between neighboring voxels, specifically for the trained condition, increased from T1 to T2. A voxelwise threshold of p < 0.05 was applied, followed by cluster correction based on permutation testing. The analysis appears to have been restricted to brain regions not damaged in any patients. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Only in L ventral occipitotemporal cortex, there was a significant increase in Local-Hreg from T1 to T2 (p = 0.028, corrected). |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (known items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | See section S2, main effects were not significant and effects appear smaller for known than trained |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Local Heterogeneity Regression Analysis (Local-Hreg) was used to identify brain regions where the heterogeneity of timecourses between neighboring voxels, specifically for the known condition, increased from T1 to T2. A voxelwise threshold of p < 0.05 was applied, followed by cluster correction based on permutation testing. The analysis appears to have been restricted to brain regions not damaged in any patients. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | T1 spelling accuracy on training items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (training items were selected for individual patients, so training item accuracy is not an appropriate measure of spelling ability) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between Local-Hreg at T1 in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and T1 spelling accuracy of training items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant positive relationship between T1 Local-Hreg and T1 spelling accuracy on training items. |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ spelling accuracy on training items (T2 vs T1) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between Local-Hreg at T1 in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and subsequent improvement in spelling accuracy of training items from T1 to T2. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant positive relationship between T1 Local-Hreg and subsequent improvement in spelling accuracy on training items from T1 to T2. |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints T1 (n = 20) |
Covariate | Subsequent Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items (T2 vs T1) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (T1 behavioral measure should be included in model) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between Local-Hreg at T1 in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and subsequent improvement in spelling accuracy of untrained items from T1 to T2. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant positive relationship between T1 Local-Hreg and subsequent improvement in spelling accuracy on untrained items from T1 to T2. |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ spelling accuracy on training items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and change in spelling accuracy of training items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant negative relationship between change in Local-Hreg and change in spelling accuracy on training items. |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and change in spelling accuracy of untrained items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant negative relationship between change in Local-Hreg and change in spelling accuracy on untrained items. |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints T2 (n = 20) |
Covariate | T2 spelling accuracy on training items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between Local-Hreg at T2 in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and T2 spelling accuracy of training items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Previous T1 Local-Hreg in L ventral occipitotemporal ROI |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | No (the ROI was defined based on change in Local-Hreg, so spurious findings could arise in the absence of a real effect) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the L ventral occipitotemporal region previously identified and T1 Local-Hreg. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant negative relationship between change in Local-Hreg and T1 Local-Hreg. |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ spelling accuracy on training items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the R AG, L PCC, and R PCC and change in spelling accuracy of training items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Spelling probe (training items) vs rest |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia with both timepoints (n = 20) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ spelling accuracy on untrained items |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | A linear mixed effects model was used to investigate the relationship between change in Local-Hreg in the R AG, L PCC, and R PCC and change in spelling accuracy of untrained items. A complex model was used in which every voxel for every patient was considered an observation, with random effects of voxel and patient, but this is not described in detail. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | Malayalam |
Inclusion criteria | Broca's aphasia or anomic aphasia; comprehension relatively preserved; "motivated for speech therapy" |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 8 (plus 3 excluded: 2 for claustrophobia; 1 for transportation issues) |
Number of control participants | 4 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (range 18-68 years; controls were younger) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 7; females: 1) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 8; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | No (6-22 weeks; patients at different subacute stages of recovery) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | WAB translated into Malayalam |
Aphasia severity | AQ range approximately 50-80 |
Aphasia type | Broca's or anomic |
First stroke only? | Not stated |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Individual lesions |
Lesion extent | Not stated |
Lesion location | 7 L MCA, 1 bilateral MCA |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—mixed |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | Neurofeedback group: T1: pre-treatment/subacute; T2: 1-5 weeks later; T3: 2-6 weeks after T1; T4: 3-11 weeks after T1; T5: 4-12 weeks after T1; T6: 5-12 weeks after T1; no training group: T1: subacute; T2: 2-12 weeks later; controls: T1: start of study; T2: 1-4 weeks later; T3: 3-5 weeks after T1; T4: 4-8 weeks after T1; T5: 7-37 weeks after T1; T6: 12-43 weeks after T1 |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | 4 patients received 4 additional sessions involving neurofeedback training, while 4 patients received treatment as usual |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Avanto 1.5 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (picture naming events consistently located between blocks) |
Design type | Mixed |
Total images acquired | probably 964 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (event timing will make conditions difficult to disentangle) |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Neurofeedback (try to activate language areas) |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Task activated L IFG and L STG in controls (Fig. 8c), but no data on other regions, and language activations were not lateralized (Fig. 9d) |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Neurofeedback (try to activate language areas) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia mean of T1, T2, T3, T4, T5, T6 (neurofeedback patients) or T1, T2 (no training patients) vs control mean |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L Broca's area (IFG pars opercularis and triangularis); (2) L Wernicke's area (pSTG); (3-4) homotopic counterparts |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Individual activations within AAL ROIs on a separate word generation localizer |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L posterior STG ↓ R IFG pars opercularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R posterior STG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Neurofeedback (try to activate language areas) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with neurofeedback training (n = 4) mean of T4, T5, T6 vs no training (n = 4) T2 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no treatment effect; second half measures rather than measures of change) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L Broca's area (IFG pars opercularis and triangularis); (2) L Wernicke's area (pSTG); (3-4) homotopic counterparts; (5) L MFG; (6) L PrCG; (7) L Rolandic operculum; (8) L insula; (9) L IFG pars orbitalis; (10) L MFG orbital; (11) L SMG; (12) L MTG; (13) L PoCG; (14) L AG; (15) L HG |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-4) individual activations within AAL ROIs on a separate word generation localizer; (5-15) AAL |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L ventral precentral/inferior frontal junction ↑ L somato-motor |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Neurofeedback (try to activate language areas) vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia mean of T1, T2, T3, T4, T5, T6 (neurofeedback patients) or T1, T2 (no training patients) vs control mean |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Signal change in L IFG and L pSTG ROIs was computed, along with functional connectivity between these ROIs. Neurofeedback values were calculated based on signal change as well as correlation between the ROIs. Group differences in neurofeedback values were compared, but not quantified statistically. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Patients received lower neurofeedback values than controls, due to lower signal changes and lower functional connectivity. |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Lesion involving left temporo-parietal cortex and sparing left frontal cortex; relatively well-recovered |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 12 (plus 2 excluded: 1 lost to follow-up; 1 did not show any sound-related neural activation in auditory cortex after sham cTBS) |
Number of control participants | 0 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 58.8 years, range 43-72 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 8; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 12; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 37.9 ± 34.8 months, range 6-122 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | AAT |
Aphasia severity | 7 mild residual aphasia, 5 recovered |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Range 11.9-176.3 cc |
Lesion location | Left temporo-parietal cortex; maximal overlap in SMG |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—chronic treatment |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1/T2/T3: chronic; sessions consisted of cTBS over left anterior IFG, cTBS over left posterior IFG, or sham; sessions at least 7 days apart in randomized order |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | CTBS |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Verio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | No* (moderate limitation) (stimulus timing not described in detail; stated duration of data acquisition substantially outside possible range of duration of stimuli) |
Design type | Block |
Total images acquired | 740 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | No (lesion impact not addressed) |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Syllable count decision |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control data in Hartwigsen et al. (2017); L-lateralized IFG but bilateral SMG |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Semantic decision |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Yes |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Control data in Hartwigsen et al. (2017); L-lateralized IFG and AG most prominent |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Syllable count decision vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia after cTBS to posterior IFG vs sham; same patients, repeated measures |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Significantly slower response times when cTBS was applied over pIFG relative to when sham cTBS was applied |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and stringent voxelwise p |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ R basal ganglia |
Findings notes | Based on Figure 4A and Table 3 |
First level contrast | Syllable count decision vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia after cTBS to posterior IFG vs after cTBS to anterior IFG; same patients, repeated measures |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Significantly slower response times when cTBS was applied over pIFG relative to when cTBS was applied over aIFG |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and stringent voxelwise p |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis |
Findings notes | Based on Table 3 |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia after cTBS to anterior IFG vs sham; same patients, repeated measures |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Somewhat (no behavioral difference) |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in reaction time did not survive correction |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and stringent voxelwise p |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L insula ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ R SMA/medial prefrontal |
Findings notes | Based on Figure 4B and Table 3 |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia after cTBS to anterior IFG vs after cTBS to posterior IFG ; same patients, repeated measures |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Yes, matched |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Behavioral data notes | Significantly slower response times when cTBS was applied over aIFG relative to when cTBS was applied over pIFG |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Voxels spared in all patients |
Correction for multiple comparisons | Clusterwise correction with with GRFT and stringent voxelwise p |
Software | SPM12 |
Voxelwise p | .001 |
Cluster extent | Based on GRFT |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L insula ↓ R insula ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | Based on Table 3 |
First level contrast | Syllable count decision vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia after cTBS to posterior IFG vs sham; same patients, repeated measures |
Covariate | Δ RT for syllable decision (cTBS to posterior IFG timepoint vs sham timepoint) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | RT is covariate |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Whole brain correlations were computed between the difference in functional activity after cTBS to posterior IFG versus sham stimulation, and the difference in reaction times on the syllable counting task under these two conditions. The resulting SPM was thresholded at voxelwise p < .001 (CDT) followed by correction for multiple comparisons based on cluster extent and GRFT using SPM12. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Upregulation of the R supramarginal gyrus after cTBS was significantly associated with slowing of RT after cTBS. This finding remained significant after including lesion volume as covariate. |
First level contrast | Semantic decision vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia after cTBS to anterior IFG vs sham; same patients, repeated measures |
Covariate | Δ RT for semantic decision (cTBS to posterior IFG timepoint vs sham timepoint) |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | RT is covariate |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Whole brain correlations were computed between the difference in functional activity after cTBS to anterior IFG versus sham stimulation, and the difference in reaction times on the semantic decision task under these two conditions. The resulting SPM was thresholded at voxelwise p < .001 (CDT) followed by correction for multiple comparisons based on cluster extent and GRFT using SPM12. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
Language | German |
Inclusion criteria | Lesion localized to frontal or temporal cortex |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 34 (plus 50 excluded: 19 lesions spanned frontal and temporal, or were subcortical, or had persisting large vessel occlusions; 31 not all three timepoints were acquired) |
Number of control participants | 17 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | Yes (8 patients were included in Saur et al. (2006); there may also be overlap with Saur et al. (2010), a study that did not meet our inclusion criteria) |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (frontal group: mean 52.3 ± 18.9 years, range 15-78 years; temporo-parietal group: mean 54.4 ± 12.7 years, range 31-76 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 25; females: 9) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (right: 31; left: 2; other: 1; not stated for controls) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (frontal group: T1 acute: mean 3.2 ± 2.0 days, range 1-7 days; T2 subacute: mean 11.9 ± 2.2 days, range 8-17 days; T3 chronic: mean 272.6 ± 88.5 days, range 181-435 days; temporo-parietal group: T1 acute: mean 1.6 ± 0.8 days, range 1-4 days; T2 subacute: mean 10.1 ± 1.7 days, range 8-13 days; T3 chronic: mean 262.5 ± 75.0 days, range 184-394 days) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Severity only |
Language evaluation | AAT including TT, comprehension composite (LRScomp) and production composite (LRSprod) were derived |
Aphasia severity | Frontal group: T1 acute: LRScomp mean 0.48 ± 0.26; T2 subacute: LRScomp mean 0.64 ± 0.21; T3 chronic: LRScomp mean 0.91 ± 0.07; temporo-parietal group: T1 acute: LRScomp mean 0.63 ± 0.32; T2 subacute: LRScomp mean 0.79 ± 0.20; T3 chronic: LRScomp mean 0.91 ± 0.13 |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Ischemic only |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Frontal group: mean 69.3 ± 34.0 cc, range 12.3-76.6 cc; temporo-parietal group: mean 54.8 ± 41.1 cc, range 6.2-108.5 cc |
Lesion location | L MCA, frontal (n = 17) or temporo-parietal (n = 17) |
Participants notes | 1630 patients screened for inclusion; frontal patients scanned later than temporal patients at T1 and T2 |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Longitudinal—recovery |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | T1 acute: 1-7 days; T2 subacute: 8-21 days; T3 chronic: > 6 months |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | Not stated |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla or Siemens Verio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 660 (20 patients; paradigm 1) or 260 (14 patients; paradigm 2) |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | No (whole brain; TE = 96 ms questionable) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) |
Control condition | Listening to reversed speech |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | In paradigm 1, responses were required in the language condition but not the control condition, making the tasks not comparable for RT |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Not stated which of the two paradigms controls were run on, but clearly L-lateralized frontal and temporal activation; bilateral MD network activation also noted |
Contrast notes | 20 patients performed paradigm 1 and 14 patients performed paradigm 2; data were combined despite some differences; unclear whether all reversed speech was included, or only reversed speech derived from plausible sentences |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Post-hoc tests comparing 2 out of the 3 time points were corrected using the Bonferroni-Holm procedure, but there is no indication that that multiple comparisons across ROIs were accounted for |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L insula ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↑ R insula |
Findings notes | Based on Figure 3; several additional regions are mentioned in text and/or Table 1 |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Post-hoc tests comparing 2 out of the 3 time points were corrected using the Bonferroni-Holm procedure, but there is no indication that that multiple comparisons across ROIs were accounted for |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis ↑ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ L anterior temporal |
Findings notes | Based on Figure 3; several additional regions are mentioned in text and/or Table 1 |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Post-hoc tests comparing 2 out of the 3 time points were corrected using the Bonferroni-Holm procedure, but there is no indication that that multiple comparisons across ROIs were accounted for |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | Based on Figure 3; several additional regions are mentioned in text and/or Table 1 |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal mean of T1, T2, T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal mean of T1, T2, T3 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↑ R IFG pars orbitalis ↑ R anterior temporal ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | Based on Table 1 |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Interactions were significant in model with all 3 time points; post-hoc sub-interactions not reported but the patterns appear clear |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Interactions were significant in model with all 3 time points; post-hoc sub-interactions not reported and patterns are not clear |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ R IFG pars triangularis ↓ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Post-hoc sub-interactions not reported but there do not appear to be any T2/T3 effects |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Test of group by time interaction not reported |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant increase in activation in perilesional ROIs |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Test of group by time interaction not reported |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | There was a significant increase in activation in perilesional ROIs |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal change in aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Test of group by time interaction not reported |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal mean of T1, T2, T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal mean of T1, T2, T3 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Test of group by time interaction not reported; this comparison is somewhat questionable given the differing extent to which frontal and temporal regions are activated in controls |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Frontal patients showed relatively greater activation in regions homotopic to their lesions |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L insula ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L insula ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ↓ L SMA/medial prefrontal ↓ L posterior STG/STS/MTG ↓ R IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L anterior temporal ↑ R IFG pars triangularis ↑ R anterior temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L insula |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; circular because patients but not controls used to define ROIs |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 13 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars opercularis ↓ L IFG pars triangularis ↓ L IFG pars orbitalis ↓ L dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Frontal patients showed reduced activation in perilesional tissue |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Frontal patients showed reduced activation in perilesional tissue |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Frontal patients showed reduced activation in perilesional tissue |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Temporal patients showed reduced activation in perilesional tissue and in regions homotopic to their lesions |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Other |
How many ROIs are there? | 2 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) perilesional tissue; (2) regions homotopic to lesions; each unique to individuals |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (2) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L IFG pars triangularis ↑ L IFG pars orbitalis other |
Findings notes | L IFG pars opercularis and orbitalis did not remain significant when lesion volume was included as a covariate; there was a significant correlation between perilesional activation and LRScomp; this did not remain significant when lesion volume was included as a covariate |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis other |
Findings notes | There was a significant correlation between perilesional activation and LRScomp |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | Did not remain significant when lesion volume was included as a covariate |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex |
Findings notes | R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex did not remain significant when lesion volume was included as a covariate |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ R anterior temporal |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L IFG pars opercularis ↑ L posterior STG/STS/MTG |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) |
Covariate | Comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal (n = 17) T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↑ L insula |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | Δ comprehension composite |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | ↓ L IFG pars triangularis |
Findings notes | Lesion volume negatively correlated with activation |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T1 |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T3 vs T2 |
Covariate | Lesion volume |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Mixed |
How many ROIs are there? | 15 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L IFG orb; (2) L IFG tri; (3) L IFG op; (4) L DLPFC; (5) L insula; (6) L ATL; (7) L PTL; (8) L SMA/dACC; (9) R L IFG orb; (10) R IFG tri; (11) R insula; (12) R DLPFC; (13) R ATL; (14) perilesional tissue; (15) regions homotopic to lesions |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | (1-13) spheres around peaks of whole brain analysis of all patients collapsing across groups and timepoints; (14) perilesional ROIs were voxels 3-15 mm from the lesion that were located in frontal or temporal regions activated by the language contrast in controls; (15) homotopic ROIs were flipped lesions |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | — |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Correlations were higher in the temporal group in the R ATL. |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Correlations were higher in the temporal group in L posterior temporal cortex and L IFG op. |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Correlations were different between groups in the R ATL, but the correlation is not reported as significant in the temporo-parietal group alone. |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) vs (aphasia temporo-parietal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and changes in LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | In the L insula, the temporo-parietal group showed a stronger correlation than the frontal group between changes in activation and changes in LRScomp. |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and changes in LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | In the L insula, the temporo-parietal group showed a stronger correlation than the frontal group between changes in activation and changes in LRScomp. |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and changes in LRScomp were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions, using interaction terms as well as the Fisher r-to-z transformation. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T1 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T1 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T2 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T2 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia frontal T3 (n = 17) vs temporo-parietal T3 (n = 17) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T2 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T1) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | (Aphasia frontal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) vs (temporo-parietal (n = 17) T3 vs T2) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Correlations between changes in activity in 15 ROIs and lesion extent were compared between patients with frontal and temporal lesions. There was no correction for multiple comparisons across the 15 ROIs. |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T1 |
Covariate | Interaction of comprehension composite by lesion size |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | To investigate why some activation-behavior relationships did not remain significant when lesion extent was included as a covariate, models were constructed looking at the relationship between activation and behavior in patients with larger and smaller lesions. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | The three regions where this applied at T1, namely perilesional cortex, L IFG op, and L IFG orb, all showed positive correlations between activation and LRScomp in patients with larger lesions, but no correlations in patients with smaller lesions. |
First level contrast | Listening to normal sentences and making a plausibility judgment (paradigm 1) or listening to normal sentences (paradigm 2) vs listening to reversed speech |
Analysis class | Longitudinal correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia T2 vs T1 |
Covariate | Interaction of Δ comprehension composite by lesion size |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | No differences in proportion of expected button presses by group or time, but behavioral data pooled across conditions |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | To investigate why some activation-behavior relationships did not remain significant when lesion extent was included as a covariate, models were constructed looking at the relationship between activation and behavior in patients with larger and smaller lesions. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | This applied to the R DLPFC in the T2 vs T1 analysis. This region showed a positive correlation between activation and LRScomp in patients with larger lesions, but no correlation in patients with smaller lesions. |