Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | No severe receptive aphasia |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 53 |
Number of control participants | 24 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (mean 62 ± 14 years, range 26-83 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | No (males: 32; females: 21; controls were mostly female, unlike patients) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 50; left: 3) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (mean 111 ± 27 days, range 84-200 days) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Comprehensive battery |
Language evaluation | CAT, QPA |
Aphasia severity | "relatively mild stroke"; 17 patients were so mild that they were not aphasic per the CAT |
Aphasia type | Not stated |
First stroke only? | No |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Lesion overlay |
Lesion extent | Mean 25.4 ± 13.5 cc, range 0.3-168.0 cc |
Lesion location | L; modest R involvement in 7 cases |
Participants notes | Prior strokes were allowed only if no aphasia resulted |
Modality | fMRI |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (Siemens Trio 3 Tesla) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | Event-related |
Total images acquired | 213 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | sparse sampling; mini-blocks of 2-4 trials |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Rest |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | No |
Control activation notes | Control data for univariate analysis in Geranmayeh et al. (2014), but note that the present paper does not describe a univariate analysis; control activations reflect speech rather than language |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Counting |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Yes |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Yes |
Control activation notes | Control data for univariate analysis in Geranmayeh et al. (2014), but note that the present paper does not describe a univariate analysis; control activations are L frontal, L pSTS, L SMA, L > R occipito-temporal |
Contrast notes | — |
Language condition | Propositional speech production |
Control condition | Target decision |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | No |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, tasks not comparable |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | No |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Unknown |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Unknown |
Control activation notes | — |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Identified using ICA in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs counting |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Identified using ICA in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | ↑ L insula ↑ L anterior cingulate ↑ R insula ↑ R anterior cingulate ↓ L IFG ↓ L inferior parietal lobule ↓ L posterior inferior temporal gyrus/fusiform gyrus |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs target decision |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Regions of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 4 |
What are the ROI(s)? | (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network; (3) cingulo-opercular network; (4) default mode network |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | Identified using ICA in controls |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No correction |
Statistical details | Circular because ROIs defined in one group |
Findings | None |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Activity was compared between pairs of ICA-derived networks. However, circularity was introduced because the networks were defined based on the control group. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Patients showed greater differential activation than controls between (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (3) cingulo-opercular network and the DMN. |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional correlation with language or other measure |
Group(s) | Aphasia |
Covariate | Appropriate information-carrying words |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | Accuracy is covariate |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | Multiple regression was used to determine whether differential activation between networks was predictive of the behavioral measure: appropriate information-carrying words. There is no issue of circularity with this analysis since it involved only individuals with aphasia. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | Differential activation between L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN was positively correlated with AICW. Differential activation between R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN was negatively correlated with AICW. |
First level contrast | Propositional speech production vs rest |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | No, different |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | Unknown, not reported |
Behavioral data notes | Difference in AICW/trial |
Type of analysis | Complex |
Statistical details | PPI analyses were used to investigate how the speech condition modulated functional connectivity between (1) L fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN; (2) R fronto-temporo-parietal network and the DMN. However, circularity was introduced because the networks were defined based on the control group. |
Findings | Other |
Findings notes | In controls, the L FTP network reduced connectivity with the DMN during speech, while the R FTP network increased connectivity with the DMN during speech. Both of these interactions were significantly decreased in patients. This was also true for contrasts 2 and 3. |