Language | UK English |
Inclusion criteria | — |
Number of individuals with aphasia | 15 |
Number of control participants | 8 |
Were any of the participants included in any previous studies? | No |
Is age reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (range 43-76 years) |
Is sex reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (males: 11; females: 4) |
Is handedness reported for patients and controls, and matched? | Yes (right: 11; left: 0) |
Is time post stroke onset reported and appropriate to the study design? | Yes (range 5-76 months) |
To what extent is the nature of aphasia characterized? | Not at all |
Language evaluation | PPT (Dutch), British picture vocabulary scale, Action for Dysphasic Adults lexical decision battery, auditory maximal pairs (an offline phoneme discrimination test) |
Aphasia severity | Not stated |
Aphasia type | Not stated, but all 6 patients with pSTS damage had single word comprehension deficits acutely |
First stroke only? | Yes |
Stroke type | Not stated |
To what extent is the lesion distribution characterized? | Extent and location |
Lesion extent | Range 0.5-14% of total brain volume |
Lesion location | 9 L but sparing pSTS, 6 L including pSTS |
Participants notes | — |
Modality | PET (rCBF) |
Is the study cross-sectional or longitudinal? | Cross-sectional |
If longitudinal, at what time point(s) were imaging data acquired? | — |
If longitudinal, was there any intervention between the time points? | — |
Is the scanner described? | Yes (CTI-Siemens ECAT EXACT HR++/966) |
Is the timing of stimulus presentation and image acquisition clearly described and appropriate? | Yes |
Design type | PET |
Total images acquired | 16 |
Are the imaging acquisition parameters, including coverage, adequately described and appropriate? | Yes (whole brain) |
Is preprocessing and intrasubject coregistration adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is first level model fitting adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Is intersubject normalization adequately described and appropriate? | Yes |
Imaging notes | — |
Language condition | Higher word rates |
Control condition | Lower word rates |
Are the conditions matched for visual demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for auditory demands? | No |
Are the conditions matched for motor demands? | Yes |
Are the conditions matched for cognitive/executive demands? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched between the language and control tasks for all relevant groups? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Are control data reported in this paper or another that is referenced? | Somewhat |
Does the contrast selectively activate plausible relevant language regions in the control group? | Somewhat |
Are activations lateralized in the control data? | Somewhat |
Control activation notes | Control activation is bilateral in primary auditory cortex and the lateral STG (Fig. 1, labels 1 and 2), but there is a left-lateralized activation in the pSTS (label 3); the scatter plots in Fig. 1 show activity-word rate curves for peak pSTS voxels in individual subjects; slopes were steeper in the left hemisphere (p < 0.05), however, the identification of these voxels is not described in sufficient detail (i.e. what was the search region?) |
Contrast notes | — |
First level contrast | Higher word rates vs lower word rates |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs control |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 555; a FWE-corrected SPM is reported of the relationship in the 6 patients with L pSTS damage (Fig. 2), however it is masked in a way that is not explained (see figure caption), and there is no direct comparison between patients with L pSTS damage and controls |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Higher word rates vs lower word rates |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with pSTS (n = 6) damage vs without pSTS damage (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Voxelwise |
Search volume | Whole brain |
Correction for multiple comparisons | No direct comparison |
Software | SPM99 |
Voxelwise p | — |
Cluster extent | — |
Statistical details | Qualitative comparison on p. 555; a FWE-corrected SPM is reported of the relationship in the 6 patients with L pSTS damage (Fig. 2), however it is masked in a way that is not explained (see figure caption), and there is no direct comparison between patients with L pSTS damage and patients with R pSTS damage |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Higher word rates vs lower word rates |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional aphasia vs control |
Group(s) | Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs control (n = 8) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R pSTS |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The peak voxel for the contrast in the R pSTS from each subject's individual analysis, but the search region is not stated |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | The controls and patients without pSTS damage were combined, however it is stated in the caption to Figure 2 that the patients with pSTS damage were significantly different to both |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |
First level contrast | Higher word rates vs lower word rates |
Analysis class | Cross-sectional between two groups with aphasia |
Group(s) | Aphasia with pSTS damage (n = 6) vs aphasia without pSTS damage (n = 9) |
Covariate | — |
Is the second level contrast valid in terms of the group(s), time point(s), and measures involved? | Yes |
Is accuracy matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no behavioral measure |
Is reaction time matched across the second level contrast? | N/A, no timeable task |
Behavioral data notes | — |
Type of analysis | Region of interest (ROI) |
ROI type | Functional |
How many ROIs are there? | 1 |
What are the ROI(s)? | R pSTS |
How are the ROI(s) defined? | The peak voxel for the contrast in the R pSTS from each subject's individual analysis, but the search region is not stated |
Correction for multiple comparisons | One only |
Statistical details | The controls and patients without pSTS damage were combined, however it is stated in the caption to Figure 2 that the patients with pSTS damage were significantly different to both |
Findings | ↑ R posterior STS |
Findings notes | — |